The consideration of intelligence has always been crucial for the diagnosis of specific learning disorders (SLDs). In particular, the hypothesis of a discrepancy between normal to high general intellectual abilities and poor academic achievement has traditionally been stressed (Mercer, Jordan, Allsopp, & Mercer, 1996). However, the traditional view of SLD has been criticized (e.g., Siegel, 1988). First of all, the dimensional distribution of academic and intellectual performances (i.e., a continuum of severity with no break points) has been emphasized, raising doubts on the use of specific cut points (Francis et al., 2005; see also Branum-Martin, Fletcher, & Stuebing, 2013). Another criticism concerns the fact that the discrepancy hypothesis treats intelligence as a unitary construct, contrasting a single, overall measure of intelligence (e.g., the full-scale intelligence quotient [FSIQ]) with achievement measures. However, many formulations of the construct of intelligence suggest that it can be better accounted for by considering different aspects (Carroll, 1993). Particularly in the case of children with SLD, using a battery of intelligence tests can help to detect strengths and weaknesses that could not emerge when a unitary IQ is considered (Giofrè & Cornoldi, 2015). In this respect, the different factor scores obtained using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003)-that is, the most widely used tool for assessing children intelligence in the Western countries (Evers et al., 2012)-can be useful. Recent research has shown that the intellectual profile of children with SLD differs from that of typically developing (TD) children. In particular, it has been shown that