2008
DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.301
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The structure of intelligence in children and adults with high functioning autism.

Abstract: Confirmatory factor analyses of the traditional 11 subtests of the Wechsler child and adult intelligence scales were accomplished for 137 children and 118 adults with high functioning autism (HFA) and for comparable age groups from the standardization samples contained in the Wechsler manuals. The objective was determining whether HFA groups produced similar best fitting models to those found in the normative samples or formed a separate "social intelligence" factor. Four-factor models incorporating a "social … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
33
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…What underlies this atypical cognitive profile remains unknown, but it may reflect reduced neural connectivity, idiosyncratic relationships between cognitive modules, specific cognitive deficits, such as theory of mind difficulties, and/or poor social learning. Nevertheless, individuals with autism demonstrate the dissociability of subtest performance purportedly underlying a general IQ factor and at least one large factor analytic study demonstrates weaker than expected relationships among IQ subtests for children and adults with high functioning autism (Goldstein et al 2008). Moreover, among developmental disorders, autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are particularly associated with savant skills (Heaton and Wallace 2004;Treffert and Wallace 2002) that are often far out of line with measured IQ.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…What underlies this atypical cognitive profile remains unknown, but it may reflect reduced neural connectivity, idiosyncratic relationships between cognitive modules, specific cognitive deficits, such as theory of mind difficulties, and/or poor social learning. Nevertheless, individuals with autism demonstrate the dissociability of subtest performance purportedly underlying a general IQ factor and at least one large factor analytic study demonstrates weaker than expected relationships among IQ subtests for children and adults with high functioning autism (Goldstein et al 2008). Moreover, among developmental disorders, autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are particularly associated with savant skills (Heaton and Wallace 2004;Treffert and Wallace 2002) that are often far out of line with measured IQ.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Altogether, 2200 participants comprised the normative sample to ensure that the standardization was based on a representative sample (Wechsler, 2003). By allowing for a frame of reference, the WISC-IV normative sample now allows for differential diagnoses and treatment decisions to be made based on the degree to which children deviate from typically developing children of the same age (Goldstein et al, 2008), and for monitoring relative progress (Koegel, Koegel, & Smith, 1997). From a behavioral approach to language development, the ABLLS and the VB-MAPP have also provided samples of normative data.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although tentatively, it might be thought that related with the results reported by Goldstein et al (2008) and Brock et al (2008) there are fewer interfactor correlations in the non-learner group which might be seen as an indicator of more modular intelligence. Likewise, Aljunied and Frederickson (2011) found significant correlations between learning gains and GEFT scores in the set of participants with good non-verbal intelligence scores, while correlations were negative for the set of participants with low non-verbal intelligence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 71%