1969
DOI: 10.2307/488713
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Structure of Japanese Baby Talk

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
14
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
2
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These studies, carried out with transcription data, not instrumental analysis, reported baby talk patterns to include consonant cluster reduction, consonant harmony (place or nasality), consonant fronting, labialization, replacement of rhotics, stopping of fricatives, and initial or final deletion. Relevantly to the present paper, coronals were reported to be palatalized in baby talk in Warlpiri (Laughren, 1984), Japanese (Chew, 1969), South Estonian (Pajusalu, 2001), and Huichol, among Wixaritari people (Grimes, 1955). In East Cree and in Warlpiri, both languages without contrastive stop voicing, stops have been reported as voiced in baby talk (Jones, 1988;O'Shannessy, 2011).…”
Section: The Nature Of Baby Talksupporting
confidence: 54%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These studies, carried out with transcription data, not instrumental analysis, reported baby talk patterns to include consonant cluster reduction, consonant harmony (place or nasality), consonant fronting, labialization, replacement of rhotics, stopping of fricatives, and initial or final deletion. Relevantly to the present paper, coronals were reported to be palatalized in baby talk in Warlpiri (Laughren, 1984), Japanese (Chew, 1969), South Estonian (Pajusalu, 2001), and Huichol, among Wixaritari people (Grimes, 1955). In East Cree and in Warlpiri, both languages without contrastive stop voicing, stops have been reported as voiced in baby talk (Jones, 1988;O'Shannessy, 2011).…”
Section: The Nature Of Baby Talksupporting
confidence: 54%
“…The underlying theoretical orientation of this research was that understanding the nature of the input to children would be critical to understanding how children develop language. A series of crosslinguistic studies thus investigated the features of caregivers' (especially mothers') talk to infants and young children, with a focus on the phonological patterns resembling immature child speech and the special baby talk lexicon (Casagrande, 1948;Chew, 1969;Cruttenden, 1994;Ferguson, 1964Ferguson, , 1977Grimes, 1955;Shankara Bhat, 1967). These studies, carried out with transcription data, not instrumental analysis, reported baby talk patterns to include consonant cluster reduction, consonant harmony (place or nasality), consonant fronting, labialization, replacement of rhotics, stopping of fricatives, and initial or final deletion.…”
Section: The Nature Of Baby Talkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, more experience with words beginning with /s/ could allow children to extract away the sound segment earlier than other, less commonly used speech sounds. In Japanese, although /s/ and /∫/ are about equally frequent, caregivers frequently palatalize their /s/ sound as a sound symbolism for smallness or cuteness in addressing young children, a process resulting in more /∫/‐like input than suggested by lexicon (Chew, 1969). Palatalization in child‐directed speech in Japanese could also account for the apparent discrepancy between fewer number of familiar /∫/‐initial words in JCDI and the earlier emergence of /∫/‐like forms in children’s speech.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…21;Hamano 1998:186-187). The two processes are also expected to share some characteristics, given the fact that mimetic vocabulary is used very frequently in child-directed speech (at a rate about five times higher than with adults; Imai, Kita, Nagumo and Okada 2008) and given the high incidence of (non-mimetic) reduplication in babytalk (Chew 1969;cf. Mazuka, Kondo, and Hayashi 2008).…”
Section: Japanese Babytalk Vs Mimetic Palatalizationmentioning
confidence: 99%