2015
DOI: 10.1017/s0952675715000068
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The syntax and prosody of apposition in Shingazidja

Abstract: This paper investigates the syntax-prosody interface with respect to apposition in Shingazidja. We examine the syntactic properties of two types of apposition (restrictive and non-restrictive). While restrictive apposition appears to form a single constituent, the syntactic data for non-restrictives are ambiguous between a single constituent analysis and an analysis in which the appositive and its anchor are syntactically separate. Prosodic data confirm the single constituent analysis for restrictive appositio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
4
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This scenario should be tested in a thorough comparative study, which should pay special attention to prosodic clues for the syntactically layered nature of Bantu NPs. In loose apposition, an anchor is typically separated from the appositive by means of a prosodic boundary, as O'Connor & Patin (2015) have demonstrated for Ngazidja G44a. Differences between types of modifiers in prosodic bonding with their head in the Bantu languages could be reflexes of such boundaries.…”
Section: Towards a Historical Explanation For The Uncommon Word Ordermentioning
confidence: 92%
“…This scenario should be tested in a thorough comparative study, which should pay special attention to prosodic clues for the syntactically layered nature of Bantu NPs. In loose apposition, an anchor is typically separated from the appositive by means of a prosodic boundary, as O'Connor & Patin (2015) have demonstrated for Ngazidja G44a. Differences between types of modifiers in prosodic bonding with their head in the Bantu languages could be reflexes of such boundaries.…”
Section: Towards a Historical Explanation For The Uncommon Word Ordermentioning
confidence: 92%
“…In such a case, as illustrated in (10) and Figure 2, neither of the two syllables is more prominent, or longer, than the other (N.B. the tone on the final syllable is blunted due to a phrasal boundary L% tone -see [22], [20] The data in (10) indicates that, other things being equal, the tone is associated with the same phonetic cues as stress, in addition to pitch. When the tone emerges on the penult, as in (9), stress and tone combine, resulting in a significant lengthening of the penultimate vowel, which becomes metrically prominent, and an intense reduction of the final vowel.…”
Section: The Tone Is Final and The Vowels Are Identicalmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…The prosodic system of Shingazidja is well described (see [12], [13], [14], [15], [7], [16], [9], [17], [18], [19], [20], [21] or [22], among others). Shingazidja has a 'privative' tone system in the sense of [23], where /H/ contrasts with Ø [20].…”
Section: /-ʤA-/mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Cette dimension post-lexicale de la LT est soutenue par un dernier argument. En shingazidja, un groupe prosodique qui ne présente pas de ton lexical se voit attribuer un ton -ou plus probablement un intonème (Patin à paraître) -sur son avant-dernière syllabe (Philippson 2005, Patin 2007, O'Connor & Patin 2015. En (17), par exemple, aucun terme ne présente de ton sous-jacent 11 ; un ton, pourtant, est inséré sur l'avant-dernière syllabe de l'énoncé.…”
Section: Présentation Du Phénomèneunclassified