1976
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.83.5.394
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The theory of achievement motivation revisited: The implications of inertial tendencies.

Abstract: The basic: theory of achievement motivation as developed by Atkinson is reviewed, and lhe implications of the incrliaUcndcncy postulate are examined. The classic theory of achievement motivation is found to be a special case of a more general theory relating lask difficulty and number of trials to performance. II is shown that the inertial-temlcncy postulate implies an asymmetric, curvilinear relationship between lask difficulty and effort, and that the degree of asymmetry is a function of the number of experi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
40
0
2

Year Published

1984
1984
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
3
40
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…We prefer to think of time spent on a task as indicating the relative allocation of resources between different tasks but not the total availability of resources. In this context, much of the data supporting the theory of achievement motivation (Atkinson, 1974;Kuhl & Blankenship, 1979;Revelle & Michaels, 1976) show consistent effects for preferences between tasks or time spent on tasks. Efforts to show differences in intensity of performance (e.g., Rocklin, 1981) are much less successful.…”
Section: Achievement Motivation and Performancementioning
confidence: 96%
“…We prefer to think of time spent on a task as indicating the relative allocation of resources between different tasks but not the total availability of resources. In this context, much of the data supporting the theory of achievement motivation (Atkinson, 1974;Kuhl & Blankenship, 1979;Revelle & Michaels, 1976) show consistent effects for preferences between tasks or time spent on tasks. Efforts to show differences in intensity of performance (e.g., Rocklin, 1981) are much less successful.…”
Section: Achievement Motivation and Performancementioning
confidence: 96%
“…In many cases, those who exert effort are more successful (Revelle & Michaels, 1976;Thomas, 1983;Van Lange et al, 1997;Wieselquist et al, 1999). Therefore, in our scenario, it was interesting to find that when effort did not lead to success, participants still found the target to be as desirable as the successful target that exerted effort.…”
Section: Successmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…When people exert effort, they are more likely to be successful in a myriad of activities (Revelle & Michaels, 1976;Thomas, 1983;Van Lange, Rusbult, Drigotas, Arriaga, Witcher, & Cox, 1997;Wieselquist et al, 1999). The effort might lead to increased time spent on that activity or increased motivation creating a better likelihood of success thus enhancing views on effort exertion.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Environmental cues at the reactive level evoke fixed action patterns, while at the routine level they evoke action tendencies which in turn elicit actions. In a negative feedback loop, these actions in turn reduce the very action tendencies that evoked them (Atkinson & Birch, 1970;Revelle & Michaels, 1976;Revelle, 1986). The reflective layer is a control layer for the two lower ones, and monitors and steers the performance of the reactive and routine levels.…”
Section: The Abcds Of Personalitymentioning
confidence: 99%