2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0992-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The time course of cognitive control implementation

Abstract: Optimally recruiting cognitive control is a key factor in efficient task performance. In line with influential cognitive control theories, earlier work assumed that control is relatively slow. We challenge this notion and test whether control also can be implemented more rapidly by investigating the time course of cognitive control. In two experiments, a visual discrimination paradigm was applied. A reward cue was presented with variable intervals to target onset. The results showed that reward cues can rapidl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

5
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Due to its non-immediate mode of operation (Seamans & Yang, 2004), dopaminergic effects would not be expected to arise instantaneously. Consistent with this, it has recently been discussed, how reward-cue information would take a couple of hundreds of milliseconds in order to take an effect (Chiew, Stanek, & Adcock, 2016; but see also Janssens, De Loof, Pourtois, & Verguts, 2016), as was possible in the present Experiment 2. In this context of task preparation, the absence of an interaction between reward and congruency in the present experiments is relevant (concerning behavioral results and the majority of pupillometry and EEG results).…”
Section: Transient Preparatory Effort Reflected In the Eegsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Due to its non-immediate mode of operation (Seamans & Yang, 2004), dopaminergic effects would not be expected to arise instantaneously. Consistent with this, it has recently been discussed, how reward-cue information would take a couple of hundreds of milliseconds in order to take an effect (Chiew, Stanek, & Adcock, 2016; but see also Janssens, De Loof, Pourtois, & Verguts, 2016), as was possible in the present Experiment 2. In this context of task preparation, the absence of an interaction between reward and congruency in the present experiments is relevant (concerning behavioral results and the majority of pupillometry and EEG results).…”
Section: Transient Preparatory Effort Reflected In the Eegsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…At the end of each block, an empty square appeared and participants had to retrieve and insert the WM load using the numerical pad of the keyboard. The Baseline Task was implemented as a basic visual discrimination task (Janssens, De Loof, Pourtois, & Verguts, 2016) in which participants had to judge whether the largest opening in a geometric figure (a square, a diamond, or a circle) was on the top or in the bottom (see Fig. 1B).…”
Section: Towards a Novel Paradigmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, with stronger control there are less errors, faster RTs, and smaller congruency effects. This occurs both proactively (e.g., because the reward at stake is higher or because the upcoming task is difficult; Janssens, De Loof, Pourtois, & Verguts, 2016;Vassena et al, 2014;Padmala & Pessoa, 2011) and reactively (e.g., in response to current trial processing difficulty or after an error or incongruent trial; Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%