2007
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194072
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The time required for perceptual (nonmotoric) processing in IOR

Abstract: Visual search is something we do routinely every day. We select a coin for the parking meter from a handful of coins, or we single out an icon from among a crowd of other icons on our computer desktop. Laboratory studies have been aimed at discovering the rules that govern the efficiency of visual search. One such rule, known as inhibition of return (IOR;Posner & Cohen, 1984), is held to be effective in preventing observers from reexamining locations that have already been searched. A rule such as this would p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition to an attentional explanation, however, perceptual and motor explanations of IOR have also been proposed (e.g., Berlucchi, ; Taylor & Klein, ). For example, in traditional cueing tasks, IOR is measured by comparing reaction times to targets appearing at a recently stimulated (i.e., cued) location to those appearing at a recently unstimulated (i.e., uncued) location, resulting in low‐level sensory processing differences that could contribute to any observed differences in response times (Berlucchi, ; Handy, Jha, & Mangun, ; Spalek, & Di Lollo, ). Some cueing studies have attempted to minimize sensory imbalances within the target display by presenting a nontarget at the same time (e.g., Pratt & Abrams, ), but this cannot eliminate the sensory imbalance resulting from the use of a peripheral cue in the preceding display.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to an attentional explanation, however, perceptual and motor explanations of IOR have also been proposed (e.g., Berlucchi, ; Taylor & Klein, ). For example, in traditional cueing tasks, IOR is measured by comparing reaction times to targets appearing at a recently stimulated (i.e., cued) location to those appearing at a recently unstimulated (i.e., uncued) location, resulting in low‐level sensory processing differences that could contribute to any observed differences in response times (Berlucchi, ; Handy, Jha, & Mangun, ; Spalek, & Di Lollo, ). Some cueing studies have attempted to minimize sensory imbalances within the target display by presenting a nontarget at the same time (e.g., Pratt & Abrams, ), but this cannot eliminate the sensory imbalance resulting from the use of a peripheral cue in the preceding display.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A similar longstanding debate centers on the nature of the inhibitory effects observed in more typical paradigms used to study IOR, such as the cuetarget and target-target paradigms. Some investigators have proposed that IOR reflects inhibition of motor processes (Taylor & Klein, 2000;Posner et al, 1985), whereas other investigators have proposed that IOR reflects the inhibition of perceptual processes (Spalek & Di Lollo, 2007;Handy, Jha, & Mangun, 1999) or the covert deployment of attention (Reuter-Lorenz, Jha, & Rosenquist, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After attention disengages from the cued location, an inhibition tendency is established to bias attention away from that location. This biased attention process thus distributes fewer attentional resources to the previously attended location and consequently impairs perceptual processing of targets at that location, resulting in slower responses to those targets (Handy & Jha, ; McDonald et al., ; McDonald, Ward, & Kiehl, ; Spalek & Di Lollo, ). The direct evidence for the attention bias theory comes from a recent event‐related potential (ERP) study by McDonald and colleagues (), in which they determined whether IOR was accompanied by a change in the attention‐sensitive N2pc component.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%