2020
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-019-00993-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The tip-of-the-tongue state bias permeates unrelated concurrent decisions and behavior

Abstract: The tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) state-the feeling of being near accessing an as yet inaccessible word from memory-is associated with cognitive bias. For example, prior work has shown that TOTs are associated with a bias toward inferring positive qualities of the unretrieved information. People are biased during TOTs to indicate that the unretrieved target has a greater likelihood of being positively valenced and to have been associated with a higher value number earlier in the experiment. Additionally, when the TO… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
14
0
2

Year Published

2021
2021
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
14
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…That is, people may merely be biased toward guessing partial attributes during TOTs relative to non-TOTs. As mentioned, TOTs have been shown to be biasing in various ways (e.g., Cleary, 2019;Cleary & Claxton, 2015), including the TOT word frequency bias shown in Experiment 1, and some of the ways in which TOTs are biasing have been shown to affect actual behavior even when TOTs are not actually relevant to the behavior in question (e.g., Cleary et al, 2020).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…That is, people may merely be biased toward guessing partial attributes during TOTs relative to non-TOTs. As mentioned, TOTs have been shown to be biasing in various ways (e.g., Cleary, 2019;Cleary & Claxton, 2015), including the TOT word frequency bias shown in Experiment 1, and some of the ways in which TOTs are biasing have been shown to affect actual behavior even when TOTs are not actually relevant to the behavior in question (e.g., Cleary et al, 2020).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Although Bahrick et al (2011) found evidence for increased first-letter reporting during TOTs than non-TOTs, it is unclear if this was under forced-responding conditions as opposed to simply instructing people to guess and, if the latter, what the role of increased typed partial information responses during TOTs may have been. In previous research where participants are instructed or encouraged to guess, they still often choose not to guess when not forced on every trial (e.g., Cleary, 2006Cleary, , 2019Cleary & Claxton, 2015;Cleary & Specker, 2007;Cleary et al, 2020Cleary et al, , 2021Cleary & Reyes, 2009); thus, examining partial attribute access under forced-responding conditions is important in order to disentangle an increased inclination to type partial information from increased accuracy.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…More recently, Cleary, Huebert, and McNeely-White (2020) found a greater inclination toward risk-taking during reported TOTs. This points toward the likelihood that TOT states will bias participants toward an inclination to take a guess on a subsequent multiple-choice set in a test-taking situation in which doing so involves risk.…”
Section: The Tip-of-the-tongue Phenomenonmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Based on that estimate, we set a cut-off date to stop running an experiment and switch to another. Based on prior studies of TOT biases (e.g., Cleary, 2019; Cleary & Claxton, 2015; Cleary et al, 2020), we aimed to run approximately 50 participants per between-subjects condition of each experiment.…”
Section: The Tip-of-the-tongue Phenomenonmentioning
confidence: 99%