2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.11.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The topography of the scalp-recorded visual N700

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Blink potentials are mainly caused by currents flowing from the positively charged cornea to the anterior forehead when the eyelids slide down (Matsuo et al, 1975;Antervo et al, 1985). Thus, blinks produce a slightly lower negativity below and a stronger positivity above the eye (Picton et al, 2000;Bender et al, 2008). During early TMS-evoked N100 (60-110 ms), the frontopolar positivity was accompanied by a smaller infraorbital negativity, a hint towards time-locked blinks in response to the TMS (single trial example in Supplementary Fig.…”
Section: Source Modellingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Blink potentials are mainly caused by currents flowing from the positively charged cornea to the anterior forehead when the eyelids slide down (Matsuo et al, 1975;Antervo et al, 1985). Thus, blinks produce a slightly lower negativity below and a stronger positivity above the eye (Picton et al, 2000;Bender et al, 2008). During early TMS-evoked N100 (60-110 ms), the frontopolar positivity was accompanied by a smaller infraorbital negativity, a hint towards time-locked blinks in response to the TMS (single trial example in Supplementary Fig.…”
Section: Source Modellingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While frontal potentials have been associated with orienting and recruitment of resources for task performance [9], [10], modality-specific encoding in visual areas during the same time interval has been proposed to represent an important short-term memory buffer [5], [6]. This late negativity over occipito-temporal areas occurs about half a second after single visual, but not auditory or somatosensory stimuli [11], [12]. Both timing and topography thus distinguish this postprocessing component from components related to cognitive or motor preparation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both timing and topography thus distinguish this postprocessing component from components related to cognitive or motor preparation. The prolonged modality-dependent processing despite the short stimulus duration of 150 ms (referred to here as ‘post-processing’ [11], [12], [14], [15], [16]) is particularly important because the effective selection of relevant aspects of a stimulus often depends on the completion of stimulus perception and evaluation. In the case of short-lasting stimuli, the selective working memory encoding of relevant stimulus characteristics must thus take place after the end of the perceptual stimulation, i.e.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations