1924
DOI: 10.1085/jgp.7.2.235
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Visual Discrimination of Intensity and the Weber-Fechner Law

Abstract: 1. A study of the historical development of the Weber-Fechner law shows that it fails to describe intensity perception; first, because it is based on observations which do not record intensity discrimination accurately, and second, because it omits the essentially discontinuous nature of the recognition of intensity differences. 2. There is presented a series of data, assembled from various sources, which proves that in the visual discrimination of intensity the threshold difference ΔI bears no … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
116
0
2

Year Published

1929
1929
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 248 publications
(126 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
8
116
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Both in intensity discrimination and in visual acuity, K for the rods is many times greater than K for the cones. The relation which this bears to the rate of dark adaptation of the two systems has already been discussed (Hecht, 1924). Here we find precisely the same difference in the K values, those for the rods being about a thousand times those for the cones.…”
Section: Comparisons With Other Visual Functionssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Both in intensity discrimination and in visual acuity, K for the rods is many times greater than K for the cones. The relation which this bears to the rate of dark adaptation of the two systems has already been discussed (Hecht, 1924). Here we find precisely the same difference in the K values, those for the rods being about a thousand times those for the cones.…”
Section: Comparisons With Other Visual Functionssupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Measurements of visual intensity discrimination are usually made with a bipartite test field of which one side has an intensity I and the other an intensity I~I. The procedure seems to involve the simultaneous and complete adaptation of the correspondingly juxtaposed retinal areas to their respective intensities, 5 whereas our critical equations depend on the initial effect 5 The formulation of intensity discrimination made ten years ago (Hecht, 1924 b; rests on this basis. It was then assumed that the eye will just discriminate between two intensities when their corresponding stationary states differ by a constant amount of photoproducts.…”
Section: Human Eyementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since 1924, when it was shown that, plotted as aI/I against log I, the data of these four observers could be superimposed on a single graph (Hecht, 1924 b), there have been some minor additions to the data. HoUaday (1927), as part of a study of glare, determined 4I/1 for a portion of the intensity range, and secured data which resemble those of Koenig and Brodhun, without, however, going to high enough intensities to confirm or deny the rise of 4I/1 found by them.…”
Section: Twenty-five Years Later Koenig and Brodhun Measured The Valumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most of these approaches incorporate in some way the complexity of human emotion, that are not considered in the framework of the ITU. Some of the human mind's complex relationships have been researched in context of QoE : Egger et al show in [8] the direct applicability of the WeberFechner Law (see [10] for a brief historical outline) to the relationship of waiting time and download experience. They proof this finding empirically for simple waiting tasks and furthermore, they also investigate the applicability of logarithmic relations between bandwidth and mean opinion score for more complex tasks like web browsing.…”
Section: Challenges Of Objective Methods For Network Related Qoementioning
confidence: 99%