“…The proximal cues usually used in the studies were traffic cones (Bullens, Nardini et al, 2010), small colored or geometrical wood pieces (Bullens, Klugkist, & Postma, 2011;Ruggiero et al, 2016), paper lanterns (Crowther et al, 2000;, rhomboid tabletop (Hupbach & Nadel, 2005), cylindrical figures (Huttenlocher & Vasilyeva, 2003), colored paper squares (Learmonth et al, 2002), bookshelves and doors (Learmonth et al, 2001), columns (Lee, Winkler-Rhoades, et al, 2012), sculpture objects (Lew et al 2006), led light figures (Nardini, Jones, et al, 2008), object pictures , toys (Waismeyer & Jacobs, 2013), colored or geometrically printed walls or curtains (Learmonth et al, 2008;Lourenco, Addy, & Huttenlocher, 2009;Lourenco & Cabrera, 2015;Nardini, Atkinson, & Burgess, 2008;Twyman et al, 2007), plastic boxes or colored glasses (Learmonth et al, 2001;Schmuckler & Jewell, 2007;, as well as the use of the environment's own geometry as an allocentric cue Lee, Sovrano, & Spelke, 2012: Yousif & Lourenco, 2017. The distal cues used were shaped neon lights (Bullens, Nardini et al, 2010), colored papers (Bullens et al, 2011), colored curtains and cardboards (Gouteux & Spelke, 2001;Hupbach & Nadel, 2005), and furniture Ribordy et al, 2013;Ribordy Lambert et al, 2017;Vasilyeva & Bowers, 2006). Studies assessing performance in outdoor tasks normally used distal cues, usually trees, swings, benches, water towers, and so forth (Mandolesi et al, 2009;.…”