2019
DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2019.1599001
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Thinking Fast and Slow: Children’s Descriptive-To-Prescriptive Tendency Under Varying Time Constraints

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
10
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
2
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although adults can be quite vocal about their prescriptive social judgments (Brady et al, 2017;Okimoto & Brescoll, 2010), some of these views only become apparent when task demands are high (or when measured implicitly; Baron & Banaji, 2006;Phillips & Cushman, 2017). For instance, in speeded judgment tasks, adults often endorse more homogeneous views of categories-similar to young children-about both familiar animal kinds (e.g., Kelemen & Rosset, 2009) and familiar social kinds (e.g., Eidson & Coley, 2014; but see also Roberts & Horii, 2019, for contrasting findings with children). Together, the results of the present study and of prior work raise the possibility that age-related changes towards increased acceptance of non-conformity relate to changes across development in the evidence required to see novel kinds as meaningful, and changes in people's beliefs about when it is appropriate to disapprove of others, rather than a decrease in prescriptive reasoning per se.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although adults can be quite vocal about their prescriptive social judgments (Brady et al, 2017;Okimoto & Brescoll, 2010), some of these views only become apparent when task demands are high (or when measured implicitly; Baron & Banaji, 2006;Phillips & Cushman, 2017). For instance, in speeded judgment tasks, adults often endorse more homogeneous views of categories-similar to young children-about both familiar animal kinds (e.g., Kelemen & Rosset, 2009) and familiar social kinds (e.g., Eidson & Coley, 2014; but see also Roberts & Horii, 2019, for contrasting findings with children). Together, the results of the present study and of prior work raise the possibility that age-related changes towards increased acceptance of non-conformity relate to changes across development in the evidence required to see novel kinds as meaningful, and changes in people's beliefs about when it is appropriate to disapprove of others, rather than a decrease in prescriptive reasoning per se.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our research made important theoretical contributions. Past research suggests that preschoolers’ descriptive-to-prescriptive reasoning is easy to elicit, is robust across cultural contexts, and powerfully influences children’s social, moral, and epistemic cognition (Roberts, Ho, et al, 2017; Roberts et al, 2018, 2019, in press; Roberts & Horii, 2019). Our research tested whether this early emerging tendency is swayed by explanations for norm violations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although adults can be quite vocal about their prescriptive social judgments (Brady et al, 2017;Okimoto & Brescoll, 2010), some of these views only become apparent when task demands are high (or when measured implicitly; Baron & Banaji, 2006;Phillips & Cushman, 2017). For instance, in speeded judgment tasks, adults often endorse more homogeneous views of categories-similar to young children-about both familiar animal kinds (e.g., Kelemen & Rossett, 2009) and familiar social kinds (e.g., Eidson & Coley, 2014; but see also Roberts & Horii, 2019, for contrasting findings with children). Adults also show subtle biases to think about natural kinds as progressing toward teleological goals (Kelemen et al, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%