2016
DOI: 10.1021/acs.jchemed.5b00992
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Thinking Like a Chemist: Development of a Chemistry Card-Sorting Task To Probe Conceptual Expertise

Abstract: An underlying goal in most chemistry curricula is to enable students to think like chemists, yet there is much evidence to suggest that students can learn to solve problems without thinking conceptually like a chemist. There are few tools, however, that assess whether students are learning to think like Ph.D. faculty, putative experts in the field. Here, we present a card-sorting task that probes how individuals organize information about problems in chemistry. Chemistry faculty tend to organize around "deep" … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

10
45
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
10
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The findings from this work illustrated the differences between novices and experts, in which novices used diverse levels of interpretation including static features (i.e., Identical structural features , and Similar properties of structure ) and process‐oriented features (i.e., Similar reaction type and Similar mechanism ), while experts primarily used the Similar mechanism level (Galloway, Leung, et al, ). These findings are consistent with the previous literature across disciplines, demonstrating that experts approached problems differently than novices, including that experts tended to attend to the conceptual or deep features within a problem (Chi et al, ; Hoskinson et al, ; Krieter et al, ; Randles & Overton, ). The present study explored how the previous findings extend to a larger context, analyzing students' categorization choices, abilities, and changes over time.…”
Section: Theoretical Framework: Information Processing Theorysupporting
confidence: 90%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…The findings from this work illustrated the differences between novices and experts, in which novices used diverse levels of interpretation including static features (i.e., Identical structural features , and Similar properties of structure ) and process‐oriented features (i.e., Similar reaction type and Similar mechanism ), while experts primarily used the Similar mechanism level (Galloway, Leung, et al, ). These findings are consistent with the previous literature across disciplines, demonstrating that experts approached problems differently than novices, including that experts tended to attend to the conceptual or deep features within a problem (Chi et al, ; Hoskinson et al, ; Krieter et al, ; Randles & Overton, ). The present study explored how the previous findings extend to a larger context, analyzing students' categorization choices, abilities, and changes over time.…”
Section: Theoretical Framework: Information Processing Theorysupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Previous work has demonstrated that novices tend to rely on surface‐level features when faced with a problem (Chi et al, ; Krieter et al, ; Mason & Singh, ). This present study found predominant sorting by surface features initially but an increase in sorting by deeper, more process‐oriented methods later in the curriculum, following explicit instruction and practice as part of the curriculum.…”
Section: Implications For Teaching and Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations