2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.langsci.2021.101398
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Thought complements in Australian languages

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A similar phenomenon is reported by McGregor (2021) for an Australian language Warrwa, where the generic verb 'say, do, think' hosts an oblique pronominal marker that can cross-reference either the addressee of a speech event or the topic spoken about (see also Spronck 2015 on Ngarinyin). With expressions of reported thought, however, it can only cross-reference the topic, and corpus data suggests that such interpretation is dispreferred with reported speech.…”
Section: Unusual Interpretation Of Addressee Markingsupporting
confidence: 67%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A similar phenomenon is reported by McGregor (2021) for an Australian language Warrwa, where the generic verb 'say, do, think' hosts an oblique pronominal marker that can cross-reference either the addressee of a speech event or the topic spoken about (see also Spronck 2015 on Ngarinyin). With expressions of reported thought, however, it can only cross-reference the topic, and corpus data suggests that such interpretation is dispreferred with reported speech.…”
Section: Unusual Interpretation Of Addressee Markingsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…This approach distinguishes our study from previous studies that focus on specific lexical expressions and translational equivalents of English mental activity verbs (Fortescue 1990;Persson 1993;Onishi 1997;Stanwood 1997;Viberg 2004, inter alia) or on specific construction types (cf. the survey of thought complements in Australian languages by McGregor 2021). One of our findings follows directly from the lack of assumption that the notion of thinking must be expressed in any particular way: we find that a significant proportion of instances of reported thought do not involve any specialized lexical items, and they instantiate a variety of construction types beyond what could be observed if data were searched for specific words or syntactic relations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…Finally, the articles have also opened up various avenues for further research, but here we single out just one. McGregor's (2021) finding that constructions with belief and thought complements are hard to discriminate from reported speech constructions in Australian languages cautions us against extrapolating what we have learned about English structures to other languages. In fact, recent research shows that even in closely related Dutch, parentheticals with limited formal variability and with schematic, speaker-related meanings (viz.…”
Section: Articles In This Collectionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…To investigate such cases crosslinguistically, they propose to use the term “extended reported speech,” to avoid conflation with the more creative usage types seen in English and similar languages. As a promising research field, fictive interaction can connect cognitive linguistics with existing typological research into speech and thought representation (e.g., Buchstaller & van Alphen, 2012; Güldemann, 2008; Güldemann & von Roncador, 2002; McGregor, 1994, 2008, 2019, 2021; Spronck & Nikitina, 2019).…”
Section: Basic Conceptual Notionsmentioning
confidence: 99%