2011
DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2011.481621
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Three layers of working memory: Focus-switch costs and retrieval dynamics as revealed by theN-count task

Abstract: Two experiments explored the process of switching items in and out of the focus of attention using a new paradigm, the N count task (adapted from Garavan, 1998; N varied from 1 to 4). This task yielded a focus size of one, indicated by a substantial focus switch cost for 2 count. Additionally, the focus switch costs in response time increased with working memory load, indicating an effortful search process occurring at a speed of about 240 ms/item. Maintaining and switching to and from a passive load did not i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
34
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
4
34
1
Order By: Relevance
“…On the one hand, this demonstrates that removal is an item-specific process, and thus supports the assumption, derived from SOB, that removal operates via unlearning of a specific item-context association. On the other hand, this finding supports the notion that people try to avoid the costs associated with switching their focus of attention (Basak & Verhaeghen, 2011;Oberauer, 2002) and also their operational mode of WM (see below and Kessler & Oberauer, in press). A precedent for the avoidance of switch costs can be found in the literature on problem solving and categorization, where people may continue to use a suboptimal strategy to avoid strategy switch costs (Kalish et al, 2005;Lemaire & Lecacheur, 2010;.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…On the one hand, this demonstrates that removal is an item-specific process, and thus supports the assumption, derived from SOB, that removal operates via unlearning of a specific item-context association. On the other hand, this finding supports the notion that people try to avoid the costs associated with switching their focus of attention (Basak & Verhaeghen, 2011;Oberauer, 2002) and also their operational mode of WM (see below and Kessler & Oberauer, in press). A precedent for the avoidance of switch costs can be found in the literature on problem solving and categorization, where people may continue to use a suboptimal strategy to avoid strategy switch costs (Kalish et al, 2005;Lemaire & Lecacheur, 2010;.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…In the outer store, representations are subject to interference and decay (Verhaeghen & Basak, 2005). Whenever an item representation stored outside the focus of attention is needed for some subsequent operation, the required element must be retrieved into the focus of attention at the expense of the item already residing there (Basak & Verhaeghen, 2011). This shunting process of swapping item representations into and out of the focus of attention is called focus-switching (Voigt & Hagendorf, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The item that is represented in the focus of attention is assumed to be in a privileged state of heightened accessibility (e.g., Basak & Verhaeghen, 2011;Cowan, 1995;McElree, 2006;Nee & Jonides, 2008;Oberauer & Refreshing in WM 7 Hein, 2012). The presumed local effect of refreshing on WM representations is thus the heightened accessibility of the just-refreshed WM representation.…”
Section: The Local Effect Of Refreshing On Wm Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%