2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Timbre-colour synaesthesia: Exploring the consistency of associations based on timbre

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
(10 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Research on crossmodal correspondences between sound and color has a long history in the field of experimental psychology. Crossmodal correspondences between color and auditory stimuli are well-established in the literature, especially in synesthetes ( Karwoski, Odbert & Osgood, 1942 ; Cytowic, 2002 ; Day, 2005 ; Ward, Huckstep & Tsakanikos, 2006 ; Hänggi et al, 2008 ; Spector & Maurer, 2009 ; Menouti et al, 2015 ; Farina, Mitchell & Roche, 2016 ). For example, most synesthetes tend to associate high pitch sounds with light colors—middle ‘C’ on a piano might be red but the note three octaves higher might be green.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Research on crossmodal correspondences between sound and color has a long history in the field of experimental psychology. Crossmodal correspondences between color and auditory stimuli are well-established in the literature, especially in synesthetes ( Karwoski, Odbert & Osgood, 1942 ; Cytowic, 2002 ; Day, 2005 ; Ward, Huckstep & Tsakanikos, 2006 ; Hänggi et al, 2008 ; Spector & Maurer, 2009 ; Menouti et al, 2015 ; Farina, Mitchell & Roche, 2016 ). For example, most synesthetes tend to associate high pitch sounds with light colors—middle ‘C’ on a piano might be red but the note three octaves higher might be green.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…In fact, long before Kandinsky came out with his suggestions concerning the crossmodal association between the sound of a trumpet and the colour scarlet (see Kandinsky, 1977 ), both Locke ( 1690 ) and Leibniz (1704/ 1896 ) had already put forward essentially the same crossmodal mapping when considering the experience of a blind man. The composer Raff also reported that he perceived the colour of the sound of the trumpet to be scarlet (other people apparently report it to be bright red; Ortmann, 1933 ), while, for Kandinsky, the sound of the tuba was also red (see also Anikin & Johansson, 2019 ; Donnell-Kotrozo, 1978 ; Ginsberg, 1923 , p. 589; Menouti et al, 2015 ; Reuter et al, 2018b ). Other contemporary researchers, meanwhile, have chosen to study the correspondences between timbre and shape (see Adeli et al, 2014 ; Gurman et al, 2021 ) or harmony (namely, consonance and dissonance) and visual roughness (Giannos et al, 2021 ; see Di Stefano & Spence, 2022 , for a review on multisensory roughness, and Di Stefano et al, 2022b for a review on consonance and dissonance) (Table 3 ).…”
Section: Putative Mechanisms Underlying Sensory Translationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(NB. Pitch change may introduce loudness difference) Turned into polar mapping between low vs. high pitch, and round vs. angular shape (Marks, 1987a ; Parise & Spence, 2012 ; Wellek, 1927 ) Timbre (No obvious organizational principle) No evidence Anikin & Johansson ( 2019 ); Donnell-Kotrozo ( 1978 ); Menouti et al ( 2015 ); Mudge ( 1920 ); Reuter et al ( 2018a ) Adeli et al ( 2014 ); Gurman et al ( 2021 ); (NB. Smith & Sera, 1992 , suggest that shape is a metathetic dimension) …”
Section: Putative Mechanisms Underlying Sensory Translationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is, though, worth stressing how many of the artists working in the field of colour-music (not to mention many of the prominent research scientists working in the area) have, over the years, been distracted by the search for crossmodal mappings between musical features and colour (and/or form) in the idiosyncratic experiences reported by synaesthetes (see Donnell-Kotrozo, 1978;Galeyev, 1976Galeyev, , 2003Itoh et al, 2017;Kandinsky, 1977;von Erhardt-Siebold, 1932;Zilczer, 1987). 11 Indeed, the florid concurrents that have so often been experienced, and reported, by coloured-hearing synaesthetes have undoubtedly helped to raise awareness/interest in crossmodal mappings between this particular pair of sensory attributes (Hänggi et al, 2008;Menouti et al, 2015). In recent decades, the growing interest in sensory substitution systems/devices (e.g., for the blind) has also drawn attention to the question of how best to 'translate' visual attributes, such as colour, into sound in an intuitively meaningful manner (see Hamilton-Fletcher et al, 2016;Marks, 1983; see also Lupton, 2018).…”
Section: On the Popularity Of Colour-sound Correspondencesmentioning
confidence: 99%