1996
DOI: 10.1007/s002650050258
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Time constraints and multiple choice criteria in the sampling behaviour and mate choice of the fiddler crab, Uca annulipes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
181
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 251 publications
(183 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
2
181
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In several ¢ddlers, sampling females are only seen in high numbers for a few days in each cycle (e.g. Uca annulipes (Backwell & Passmore 1996); Uca pugilator (Christy 1978)). These periods of peak sampling activity seem to coincide with the point in the cycle when females must mate if there is to be su¤cient time for subsequent embryonic development before larval release at the next nocturnal spring tide.…”
Section: (B) Female Behaviourmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In several ¢ddlers, sampling females are only seen in high numbers for a few days in each cycle (e.g. Uca annulipes (Backwell & Passmore 1996); Uca pugilator (Christy 1978)). These periods of peak sampling activity seem to coincide with the point in the cycle when females must mate if there is to be su¤cient time for subsequent embryonic development before larval release at the next nocturnal spring tide.…”
Section: (B) Female Behaviourmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Females that burrow mate must ¢rst leave their own burrow and wander on the surface. While wandering, they sequentially enter and leave the burrows of several males (`sampling') before remaining in the chosen male's burrow (Christy & Schober 1994;Backwell & Passmore 1996). Surface mating occurs when a female is approached by a male neighbour or a wandering, burrowless male.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, once a male has directed claw waving to a female, she may be more likely to approach if his burrow is marked with a hood (Christy, 1988b;Crane, 1975;von Hagen, 1968). Finally, hoods may signal either male or burrow quality (Backwell and Passmore, 1996;Backwell et al, 1995;Christy, 1983Christy, , 1987 and increase the probability that a female will mate with a male once she reaches his burrow. Thus, hood builders may have higher mating rates because they are more likely to encounter and court females, to attract the females they court, and to mate with the females that they attract.…”
Section: Effects Of Hoods On Courtshipmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when a species colonizes a new area that is already occupied by closely related species, females may initially be courted pri-marily by heterospecific males and only rarely encounter a conspecific male because of their low relative abundance. In the laboratory, female acquiescence to mating with a heterospecific male is often seen in no-choice breeding experiments (Crapon de Caprona 1986;Seehausen et al 1997; for a field example see also Lushai et al 2005), and females often become less selective when males with preferred phenotypes are absent (Bakker and Milinski 1991;Backwell and Passmore 1996;Bateman et al 2001;Luttbeg et al 2001). Many females may acquiesce to heterospecific matings in this situation and produce fewer or less-viable offspring than females that secure matings with conspecifics.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%