1995
DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)98945-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Time course of visual extrapolation accuracy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

2
57
1

Year Published

1998
1998
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
2
57
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, to use CME, a temporal process would seem necessary to update the object's extrapolated position (and corresponding imagery) at each point in time until it reaches the end point. Alternatively, observers may compute the object's hidden time (TTC) from the ratio of exposed distance to hidden distance and the duration of the object's visible motion; the clock would count the latter duration and the hidden time (Rosenbaum, 1975; see also Lyon & Waag, 1995). Lyon and Waag (1995) classified hypotheses about the mechanisms that underlie the ability to extrapolate motion as either tracking hypotheses or timing hypotheses.…”
Section: Cognitive Motion Extrapolation and Cognitive Clocking In Prementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, to use CME, a temporal process would seem necessary to update the object's extrapolated position (and corresponding imagery) at each point in time until it reaches the end point. Alternatively, observers may compute the object's hidden time (TTC) from the ratio of exposed distance to hidden distance and the duration of the object's visible motion; the clock would count the latter duration and the hidden time (Rosenbaum, 1975; see also Lyon & Waag, 1995). Lyon and Waag (1995) classified hypotheses about the mechanisms that underlie the ability to extrapolate motion as either tracking hypotheses or timing hypotheses.…”
Section: Cognitive Motion Extrapolation and Cognitive Clocking In Prementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, observers may compute the object's hidden time (TTC) from the ratio of exposed distance to hidden distance and the duration of the object's visible motion; the clock would count the latter duration and the hidden time (Rosenbaum, 1975; see also Lyon & Waag, 1995). Lyon and Waag (1995) classified hypotheses about the mechanisms that underlie the ability to extrapolate motion as either tracking hypotheses or timing hypotheses. Timing hypotheses involve a central timing mechanism that clocks the object's visible motion; this elapsed time is the basis for how long the observer waits before responding.…”
Section: Cognitive Motion Extrapolation and Cognitive Clocking In Prementioning
confidence: 99%
“…1a). Another variant requires the participant to judge whether the target reappeared from occlusion too early or too late (Bennett & Benguigui, 2013;DeLucia & Liddell, 1998;Jonikaitis, Deubel, & de'Sperati, 2009;Lyon & Waag, 1995;O'Reilly, Mesulam, & Nobre, 2008). This is sometimes called an 'interruption paradigm' (see Fig.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fact that stimulus history biases perceptual judgments is well-known. Broadly speaking, it appears in two ways-biases to (or away from) the mean of a stimulus distribution (1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9), referred to as central-tendency biases, and biases to (or away from) recently observed stimuli (10)(11)(12)(13)(14)(15)(16), referred to as n − 1 biases.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We tested this prediction for perceptual estimates of object speed used to plan hand movements to intercept a moving object (6,(14)(15)(16). Most previous studies of perceptual biases have used some form of perceptual report of the stimulus dimension of interest.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%