Behavioral Economics of Preferences, Choices, and Happiness 2016
DOI: 10.1007/978-4-431-55402-8_9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Time Discounting and Smoking Behavior: Evidence from a Panel Survey

Abstract: By using a panel survey of Japanese adults, we show that smoking behavior is associated with personal time discounting and its biases, such as hyperbolic discounting and the sign effect, in the way that theory predicts: smoking depends positively on the discount rate and the degree of hyperbolic discounting and negatively on the presence of the sign effect. Positive effects of hyperbolic discounting on smoking are salient for naïve people, who are not aware of their self‐control problem. By estimating smoking … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0
3

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
(102 reference statements)
0
18
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Although averaging over discount factors likely reduces measurement error of our time preference variables, concerns regarding the validity of these measures remain because of the hypothetical nature of financial tradeoffs. Kang and Ikeda (), however, report ample evidence that time preferences elicited through incentivized economic experiments do not differ systematically from those obtained through hypothetical questions (p. 1448). But then, elicited time preferences should be correlated with variables that are known to be affected by time preferences, most importantly health and education (Fuchs, ).…”
Section: Empirical Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although averaging over discount factors likely reduces measurement error of our time preference variables, concerns regarding the validity of these measures remain because of the hypothetical nature of financial tradeoffs. Kang and Ikeda (), however, report ample evidence that time preferences elicited through incentivized economic experiments do not differ systematically from those obtained through hypothetical questions (p. 1448). But then, elicited time preferences should be correlated with variables that are known to be affected by time preferences, most importantly health and education (Fuchs, ).…”
Section: Empirical Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Grignon (2009) find present bias is not correlated with starting smoking but is negatively correlated with quitting smoking. The empirical link between present bias and smoking is also found by Burks et al (2012) and Kang and Ikeda (2013). We expect that individuals who smoked in 2000 are more likely to have present bias, and are more likely to quit and less likely to start participating in physical activity later in the sample.…”
Section: Results On Present Biasmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…Our data do not have directly elicited time preference measures or a clean "natural experiment" for habit formation. We draw from studies using directly elicited time preference measures and behavioral outcomes (Burks et al (2012), Grignon (2009), Kang and Ikeda (2013), and Meier and Sprenger (2010)), and use smoking and the accumulation of credit card debt to proxy for present bias.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations