2018
DOI: 10.1177/1747021817740808
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Time of day effects on the use of distraction to minimise forgetting

Abstract: Young adults typically show a substantial advantage on explicit memory tasks compared to older adults, especially when memory is tested using free recall (e.g., Craik, 1986; Craik & McDowd, 1987). It was particularly surprising then, that a study that capitalised on age-related declines in cognitive control could eliminate age differences in free recall (Biss, Ngo, Hasher, Campbell, & Rowe, 2013). The successful manipulation in that study re-exposed some of the originally learned words as distraction during th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(106 reference statements)
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A major caveat to this finding is that our sample consisted predominantly of neutral chronotypes ( S2 Table ), whereas studies reporting chronotype-related effects typically sample more densely from extreme morning and evening types [ 34 , 46 , 63 , 64 ]. Though neutral type young adults often fail to show time of day effects [ 59 ], morning testing has been classified as a non-optimal time for both evening and neutral type young adults [ 35 , 91 ], so it is likely the morning constituted a non-optimal time for the vast majority of our sample (we had very few morning types). To examine whether an evening benefit could emerge in this task, future work with strong morning types, which could be achieved by sampling older adults [ 39 , 46 ], would be informative.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A major caveat to this finding is that our sample consisted predominantly of neutral chronotypes ( S2 Table ), whereas studies reporting chronotype-related effects typically sample more densely from extreme morning and evening types [ 34 , 46 , 63 , 64 ]. Though neutral type young adults often fail to show time of day effects [ 59 ], morning testing has been classified as a non-optimal time for both evening and neutral type young adults [ 35 , 91 ], so it is likely the morning constituted a non-optimal time for the vast majority of our sample (we had very few morning types). To examine whether an evening benefit could emerge in this task, future work with strong morning types, which could be achieved by sampling older adults [ 39 , 46 ], would be informative.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, related research investigating individual differences in cognitive control-a precursor to being able to sustain attention-finds that age groups known to have lower cognitive control tend to learn more about distractors, such as older adults (Amer & Hasher, 2014;Biss, Rowe, Weeks, Hasher, & Murphy, 2018;Campbell, Hasher, & Thomas, 2010;Campbell, Healey, Lee, Zimerman, & Hasher, 2012;Kim, Hasher, & Zacks, 2007;Rowe, Valderrama, Hasher, & Lenartowicz, 2006;Schmitz, Cheng, & De Rosa, 2010;Weeks, Biss, Murphy, & Hasher, 2016) and children (Deng & Sloutsky, 2016;Plebanek & Sloutsky, 2017). Further, younger adults show improved distractor learning during off-peak times of day when they have low cognitive control (Ngo, Biss, & Hasher, 2018). And in a final key example, a correlated flanker paradigm was used to show that younger adults with high trait impulsivity-a trait associated with low control (Cools, Sheridan, Jacobs, & D'Esposito, 2007;Logan, Schachar, & Tannock, 1997)were better at learning the relationships between flankers (which they were instructed to ignore) and goal-relevant targets than those with low impulsivity (Landau, Elwan, Holtz, & Prinzmetal, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A major caveat to this finding is that our sample consisted predominantly of neutral chronotypes ( Supplementary Table 3), whereas studies reporting chronotype-related effects typically sample more densely from extreme morning and evening types (May, 1998;May et al, 2005;Simor & Polner, 2017;Wieth & Zacks, 2011). Though neutral type young adults often fail to show time of day effects (May & Hasher, 2017), morning testing has been classified as a non-optimal time for both evening and neutral type young adults (Ngo et al, 2018;Ngo & Hasher, 2017), so it is likely the morning constituted a non-optimal time for the vast majority of our sample (we had very few morning types). To examine whether an evening benefit could emerge in this task, future work with strong morning types, which could be achieved by sampling older adults (May et al, 2005;Yoon et al, 1999), would be informative.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%