2010
DOI: 10.1007/bf03395696
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Time of Semester as a Factor in Participants’ Obedience to Instructions to Perform an Aversive Task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, the initial data were collected in the last 3 weeks of the fall semester and the second data collection was early, the following spring semester. Research has found participants at the end of a semester perform more poorly on repetitive and aversive tasks (Navarick & Bellone, 2010); tend to be higher in impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, and need for power (Zelenski, Rusting, & Larsen, 2003); and tend to be in an ego-depleted state that impairs their engagement in effortful, regulated processing (Casa de Calvo & Reich, 2007). So while collecting the corrected control group we also collected 18 experimental group participants who replaced randomly selected participants from the fall.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the initial data were collected in the last 3 weeks of the fall semester and the second data collection was early, the following spring semester. Research has found participants at the end of a semester perform more poorly on repetitive and aversive tasks (Navarick & Bellone, 2010); tend to be higher in impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, and need for power (Zelenski, Rusting, & Larsen, 2003); and tend to be in an ego-depleted state that impairs their engagement in effortful, regulated processing (Casa de Calvo & Reich, 2007). So while collecting the corrected control group we also collected 18 experimental group participants who replaced randomly selected participants from the fall.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In terms of task performance, early participants attempt more items on symbol substitution tasks (Richter et al, 1981), scan more items in a digit scanning task (Richert & Ward, 1976), and spend more time completing difficult items on an anagram task (Hom, 1987). Participants who sign up late in semester are also more likely to withdraw from an aversive task, when given the chance, than those who sign up early (Navarick & Bellone, 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The locus could be internal-related to the inherent quality of the participant, or externalrelated to environmental pressures. To give an example: A participant who engages in a study late in semester may perform poorly on a task (Hom, 1987) or withdraw (Navarick & Bellone, 2010) because of an inherent lack of motivation or external pressures such as looming essays and exams. Support for the internal account comes from research showing differences between early and late participants in personality traits such as personal structure (Roman, Moskowitz, Stein, & Eisenberg, 1995) and compliance (Aviv, Zelenski, Rallo, & Larsen, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Third, because the present study isolated the effects of pain, the potential influence of salient psychosocial variables (e.g., anxiety, depression, fatigue, hope, and social support) that could impact performance was not measured. Research shows that college students high in hope are more tolerant of pain than their less hopeful peers (Snyder et al, 2005), and students who volunteer early in the school year have higher hope levels than students who volunteer late in the semester (Navarick & Bellone, 2010). However, presumably, effects of the construct of hope would be randomly distributed across both the control group and pain group, such that observed differences between the two groups would be attributable to effects of pain rather than differences in level of hope.…”
Section: Strengths and Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 89%