2006
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-006-0810-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Time-on-task effect in pseudoneglect

Abstract: Neurologically normal subjects systematically misbisect space during visual line-bisection or similar tasks, generally erring to the left of the veridical center when bisecting horizontal lines, a phenomenon referred to as pseudoneglect. This phenomenon is usually interpreted as enhanced attention toward the left hemispace resulting in an overestimation of the leftward extent of a line. While most studies have examined the role of attention in spatial bias using spatial cueing methods in bisection tasks, Manly… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

2
42
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
2
42
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Spatial biases have subsequently been observed on several measures, including both manual and computerized line bisection (i.e., Jewell & McCourt, 2000;McCourt & Jewell, 1999), cancellation tasks (Vingiano, 1991), the landmark task (Dufour, Touzalin, & Candas, 2007), the greyscales task (Nicholls, Bradshaw, & Mattingley, 1999), and measures looking at judgments of numerosity, size (Nicholls et al, 1999) and distance (Krupp, Robinson, & Elias, in press). Lateral biases have also been identified on more real-world behaviours, such as passing through a doorway (Nicholls, Loftus, Mayer, & Mattingley, 2007;Nicholls, Loftus, Orr, & Barre, 2008), choosing a seat in the theatre (Okubo, 2010), completing a route following task (Thomas, Stuckel, Gutwin, & Elias, 2009) or multiple-choice questionnaire response patterns (Nicholls, Orr, Okubo, & Loftus, 2006).…”
Section: List Of Tablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Spatial biases have subsequently been observed on several measures, including both manual and computerized line bisection (i.e., Jewell & McCourt, 2000;McCourt & Jewell, 1999), cancellation tasks (Vingiano, 1991), the landmark task (Dufour, Touzalin, & Candas, 2007), the greyscales task (Nicholls, Bradshaw, & Mattingley, 1999), and measures looking at judgments of numerosity, size (Nicholls et al, 1999) and distance (Krupp, Robinson, & Elias, in press). Lateral biases have also been identified on more real-world behaviours, such as passing through a doorway (Nicholls, Loftus, Mayer, & Mattingley, 2007;Nicholls, Loftus, Orr, & Barre, 2008), choosing a seat in the theatre (Okubo, 2010), completing a route following task (Thomas, Stuckel, Gutwin, & Elias, 2009) or multiple-choice questionnaire response patterns (Nicholls, Orr, Okubo, & Loftus, 2006).…”
Section: List Of Tablesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This left bias has been observed on several tasks, including line bisection (Luh, 1995;McCourt & Jewell, 1999), the landmark task (Dufour et al, 2007) and the greyscales task (Nicholls et al, 1999;Thomas & Elias, 2010). There are two types of line bisection tasks: manual line bisection (Luh, 1995), where participants use a stylus to bisect a line and perceptual line bisection (i.e., Dufour, et al, 2007;McCourt & Jewell, 1999;Rolfe et al, 2008), where participants are presented with a pre-transected line and asked to make a judgment regarding the location of the transection mark.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 3 more Smart Citations