2018
DOI: 10.1037/emo0000380
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Timing affect: Dimension-specific time-based expectancy for affect.

Abstract: Affective information in our environment is often predictable by time; for example, positive answers are typically given faster than negative ones. Here we demonstrate, for the first time, that humans can implicitly adapt to time-based affect predictability. Participants were asked to categorize words, with the words' irrelevant valences being predictable by the timing of their occurrence. Adaptation to this pattern became evident by better performance for typical combinations of time and valence, relative to … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 178 publications
(208 reference statements)
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, after hearing a knock on the door, it is likely that someone enters the room, but the longer the door stays closed, we start to expect that they did not hear us inviting them in. TBEEs have mainly been investigated with simple visual shapes, coloured numbers, and affective information (Aufschnaiter, Kiesel, Dreisbach, et al, 2018 ; Aufschnaiter, Kiesel, & Thomaschke, 2018 ; Kunchulia et al, 2017 ; Thomaschke et al, 2018 ; Thomaschke & Dreisbach, 2015 ; Thomaschke et al, 2011 ; Volberg & Thomaschke, 2017 ), by, for example, presenting a circle with 80% probability after a short cue–target delay (e.g., 600 ms), while a presentation of a square is 80% probable after a long cue–target delay (e.g., 1400 ms). Note that in these experiments each time point of target occurrence typically has the same likelihood (while they are unevenly distributed in TE studies).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, after hearing a knock on the door, it is likely that someone enters the room, but the longer the door stays closed, we start to expect that they did not hear us inviting them in. TBEEs have mainly been investigated with simple visual shapes, coloured numbers, and affective information (Aufschnaiter, Kiesel, Dreisbach, et al, 2018 ; Aufschnaiter, Kiesel, & Thomaschke, 2018 ; Kunchulia et al, 2017 ; Thomaschke et al, 2018 ; Thomaschke & Dreisbach, 2015 ; Thomaschke et al, 2011 ; Volberg & Thomaschke, 2017 ), by, for example, presenting a circle with 80% probability after a short cue–target delay (e.g., 600 ms), while a presentation of a square is 80% probable after a long cue–target delay (e.g., 1400 ms). Note that in these experiments each time point of target occurrence typically has the same likelihood (while they are unevenly distributed in TE studies).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The flanker interference was higher when the interval indicated low conflict proportions, which suggests that the interval can act as a contextual cue for attentional interference adjustment. Also, there is evidence that attention can be biased by implicit temporal regularities even when these regularities are uninformative about the target or irrelevant regarding the current task (Thomaschke et al, 2018; Yu & Zhao, 2015; Zhao et al, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Time-based expectancy is typically induced by correlations between a foreperiod and targets (Thomaschke & Dreisbach, 2013;Volberg & Thomaschke, 2017;Wagener & Hoffmann, 2010). Moreover, time-based expectancy has already been demonstrated in other domains such as visual stimulus perception (Thomaschke et al, 2016), attentional adjustment to conflict contingencies (Wendt, & Kiesel, 2011), language processing (Roberts & Francis, 2013;Roberts et al, 2011;Roberts & Norris, 2016), human-machine interaction (Shahar et al, 2012;Thomaschke & Haering, 2014) or the perception of emotional word valence (Thomaschke et al, 2018). These studies typically examined timebased expectancy in single tasks.…”
Section: Time-based Expectancymentioning
confidence: 99%