The rate of a reinforced response is conceptualized as a composite of engagement bouts (visits) and responding during visits. Part 1 of this paper describes a method for estimating the rate of visit initiations and the average number of responses per visit from log survivor plots: the proportion of interresponse times (IRTs) longer than some elapsed time (log scale) plotted as a function of elapsed time. In Part 2 the method is applied to IRT distributions from rats that obtained food pellets by nose poking a lighted key under various multiple schedules of reinforcement. As expected, total response rate increased as a function of (a) increasing the rate of reinforcement (i.e., variableinterval [VI] 4 min vs. VI 1 min), (b) increasing the amount of the reinforcer (one food pellet vs. four pellets), (c) increasing the percentage of reinforcers that were contingent on nose poking (25% vs. 100%), and (d) requiring additional responses after the end of the VI schedule (i.e., adding a tandem variable-ratio [VR] 9 requirement). The first three of these variables (relative reinforcement) increased the visit-initiation rate. The tandem VR, in contrast, increased the number of responses per visit. Thus, variables that have similar effects on total response rate can be differentiated based on their effects on the components of response rate.Key words: response rate, visits, bouts, relative reinforcement, tandem variable ratio, key poke, ratsThe rate of a reinforced response usually is calculated by dividing the total number of responses by the time available for the response. This method makes most sense if all instances of the response are functionally equivalent. But there are grounds for thinking that they might not be, at least not under some widely studied conditions. According to one tradition, reinforced responding is better thought of as composed of periods of engagement in the reinforced activity (i.e., visits) alternating with periods of disengagement (Blough, 1963;Gilbert, 1958;Mechner, 1992;Mellgren & Elsmore, 1991;Nevin, 1992;Nevin & Baum, 1980;Pear & Rector, 1979;Premack, 1965;Shull, 1991).Although conventional recording equipment typically cannot distinguish visit initiations from responses that occur during the visit (but see Mechner, 1992;Nevin, 1992;Pear & Rector, 1979), these two kinds of reThis research was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (IBN-9511934). Portions of these data were presented at the annual meetings of the American Psychological Association (1996) and the Association for Behavior Analysis (1999). Scott Gaynor is now at Western Michigan University.Correspondence concerning this paper should be directed to R. L. Shull, Department of Psychology, Box 26164, University of North Carolina-Greensboro, Greensboro, North Carolina 27402-6164 (E-mail: rlshull@uncg.edu).sponse might nevertheless be controlled by different variables. If so, total response rate would be a composite measure of performance rather than a unitary one. Similar changes in total response rate could res...