2011
DOI: 10.1002/ajp.20929
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Toward a cross‐species neuroscientific understanding of the affective mind: do animals have emotional feelings?

Abstract: Do we need to consider mental processes in our analysis of brain functions in other animals? Obviously we do, if such BrainMind functions exist in the animals we wish to understand. If so, how do we proceed, while still retaining materialistic-mechanistic perspectives? This essay outlines the historical forces that led to emotional feelings in animals being marginalized in behavioristic scientific discussions of why animals behave the way they do, and why mental constructs are generally disregarded in modern n… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
35
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
1
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Working memory capacity and long-term learning and memory, for example, are important for learning associations between particular behavioural strategies and hunting situations (Packer and Pusey 1997;McMorris et al 2010). In addition, animals' decisions can be strongly influenced by emotion, as can other cognitive processes such as learning and memory (de Waal 2011;Panksepp 2011). Evidence suggests that animals' ability to show restraint when excited may contribute to the success of cooperation.…”
Section: Implications For Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Working memory capacity and long-term learning and memory, for example, are important for learning associations between particular behavioural strategies and hunting situations (Packer and Pusey 1997;McMorris et al 2010). In addition, animals' decisions can be strongly influenced by emotion, as can other cognitive processes such as learning and memory (de Waal 2011;Panksepp 2011). Evidence suggests that animals' ability to show restraint when excited may contribute to the success of cooperation.…”
Section: Implications For Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Rodents emit USVs in the 50–55 kHz and 22–28 kHz ranges, which are reliably associated with positive and negative emotional states, respectively [1517]. USVs have received increased attention in drug abuse studies because administration of cocaine [18], amphetamine [19] and drug-associated cues [20, 21] increase 50–55 kHz frequency-modulated (FM) USV emissions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Selectively bred alcohol-preferring P rats display a behavioral phenotype akin to the diagnostic criteria for AUDs and thus provide an excellent model of alcoholism (Bell et al, 2006, Bell et al., 2011, Bell et al, 2012, Bell et al, 2014). Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) emitted by rodents are social signals revealing affective states and are typically categorized into two main types: 50–55 kHz frequency-modulated (FM; positive-affect related) and 22–28 kHz (negative-affect related) USVs (Burgdorf et al, 2008, Burgdorf et al, 2011, Panksepp, 2011). Ascending mesolimbic cholinergic (Brudzynski, 1994) and dopaminergic pathways (Burgdorf et al, 2007) mediate 22–28 and 50–55 kHz USV emissions, respectively, and are hypothesized to concurrently initiate positive and negative emotional states (Bihari et al, 2003, Brudzynski, 2009, Brudzynski, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%