2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Based Metaplasticity Protocols in Working Memory

Abstract: These results suggest that the polarity effects of tDCS on working memory are dependent on the previous level of activity of the recruited neural population.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
24
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
1
24
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus it is possible that unihemispheric tDCS was able to increase these outputs to subcortical regions, which in turn led to slower, but more accurate responses. These tDCS effects on accuracy but not response time have already been showed in the past (Bolognini et al, 2010; Carvalho et al, 2015; Fregni et al, 2005; Zaehle et al, 2011). Accuracy increase has been thought to represent a top down control mechanism.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus it is possible that unihemispheric tDCS was able to increase these outputs to subcortical regions, which in turn led to slower, but more accurate responses. These tDCS effects on accuracy but not response time have already been showed in the past (Bolognini et al, 2010; Carvalho et al, 2015; Fregni et al, 2005; Zaehle et al, 2011). Accuracy increase has been thought to represent a top down control mechanism.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…This suggests that the effects of unihemispheric tDCS on proactive control were task specific. This specificity of tDCS has been shown extensively in the literature, and is thought to be dependent on the activated network, the extension of its activation, its resting state, or the level of cognitive demands (Carvalho et al, 2015; Ehlis et al, 2016; Gill et al, 2015; Hsu et al, 2016). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This highlights that caution is required when interpreting cognitive findings. Also, little is known about the additive effects of tDCS and cognitive tasks (Andrews et al 2011; Miniussi et al 2013; Carvalho et al 2014). Thus, further studies are needed to explicitly compare the effects of cognitive assessment during and after stimulation.…”
Section: Monitoring Functional Effects Of Tesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, further studies are needed to explicitly compare the effects of cognitive assessment during and after stimulation. Finally, cognitive tDCS protocols should consider the possibility that differences in polarity, current intensity, electrode size, time of stimulation and volunteer group lead to different modulatory effects (Boggio et al 2006; Jacobson et al 2012; Kuo and Nitsche 2012; Carvalho et al 2014). For example, a positive effect on working memory in healthy young participants can be obtained with 1mA tDCS (Fregni et al 2005) whereas the same effect is only observed with 2mA stimulation in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Boggio et al 2006).…”
Section: Monitoring Functional Effects Of Tesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…What appears to be critical to induce meta-plasticity with tDCS is to initiate the second round of stimulation during the after-effects of the first, a window of time which typically lasts up to an hour after stimulation (Nitsche et al 2003). Behaviorally, meta-plastic protocols have also accentuated or prolonged effects on both motor learning (Bastani and Jaberzadeh 2014;Christova et al 2015) and WM (Carvalho et al 2015). In all these cases, it appears that inter-session breaks of approximately 20-30 min produce stronger effects than much shorter (3-5 min) or much longer (3-24 h) breaks, suggesting the existence of a non-linear optimum.…”
Section: Spacing Of Stimulationmentioning
confidence: 99%