2015
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4292-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual rhythmic movement coordination: transfer is a function of the task dynamic

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
48
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
3
48
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This ecological task-dynamics analysis has so far been developed most completely in the context of coordinated rhythmic movement (Bingham, 2001, 2004a, 2004b, Golonka & Wilson, 2012, 2019bSnapp-Childs, Wilson, & Bingham, 2011;Snapp-Childs, Wilson, & Bingham, 2015). The next section will briefly review that work and then set up the current study as the next step in the research programme.…”
Section: Transfer Of Task Dynamical Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This ecological task-dynamics analysis has so far been developed most completely in the context of coordinated rhythmic movement (Bingham, 2001, 2004a, 2004b, Golonka & Wilson, 2012, 2019bSnapp-Childs, Wilson, & Bingham, 2011;Snapp-Childs, Wilson, & Bingham, 2015). The next section will briefly review that work and then set up the current study as the next step in the research programme.…”
Section: Transfer Of Task Dynamical Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is the proportion of time people spent coordinating at the required phase (within an error bandwidth, typically set to 20°). Proportion-time-on target, therefore, indicates how successful participants are at coordinating at the required relative phase (Wilson et al, 2010; Snapp-Childs et al, 2011, 2015). This measure was not used in our primary analysis because our control task has no target relative phase (meaning it is not possible to compute proportion-time-on-target for the control condition).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In what way two tasks need to be related so that people can transfer what they have learned from one to the other is largely an open question (e.g., Snapp-Childs et al, 2015). However, if two tasks share most 3 One might suspect that at some point in the auction, the participants anticipated that the "winners" would not be forced to pay the full amount bid (which is, indeed, what happened: they did not have to pay the full $2,000).…”
Section: Learning From Different Types Of Experiencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Direct experience exposes individuals to all aspects of the situation, including both hot competitive escalation and its destructive consequences. Because the task in which individuals learn and the task they encounter afterwards are identical, the conditions for knowledge transfer are ideal (Snapp‐Childs, Wilson, & Bingham, ; Thorndike, ). Nevertheless, prior research has shown that after losing in a competition, people often set themselves a more challenging goal (Buser, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation