2003
DOI: 10.1017/s0952675703004482
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Turkish stress: a review

Abstract: This work evaluates an argument recently made in these pages by Kabak & Vogel (2001) to the effect that the analysis of Turkish which they develop is superior on theoretical grounds to that of past accounts. Kabak & Vogel explicitly contrast their account to that offered in two recent, comprehensive discussions of Turkish stress by Inkelas & Orgun (1998) and Inkelas (1999). Careful consideration of the data discussed by Kabak & Vogel and by Inkelas & Orgun, as well as some additional data introduced in this pa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
33
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
1
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The trochee is characterized by one stressed syllable followed by at least one relatively weaker syllable, while the iamb displays the opposite metric pattern, namely at least one unstressed syllable followed by a stressed one (Hayes, 1985). German and English provide examples of trochaic languages, while Turkish and French are iambic (Eisenberg, 1991; Inkelas and Orgun, 2003). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The trochee is characterized by one stressed syllable followed by at least one relatively weaker syllable, while the iamb displays the opposite metric pattern, namely at least one unstressed syllable followed by a stressed one (Hayes, 1985). German and English provide examples of trochaic languages, while Turkish and French are iambic (Eisenberg, 1991; Inkelas and Orgun, 2003). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whereas German and English share rhythmic classification and metric preferences (Pike, 1945; Eisenberg, 1991; Cummins and Port, 1998), Turkish and German represent rather an interesting contrast when considering their respective rhythmic properties. While German is a stress-timed language with a metric preference for the trochee, Turkish is syllable-timed and uses the iamb as its default metric pattern (Eisenberg, 1991; Grabe and Low, 2002; Inkelas and Orgun, 2003; Nazzi and Ramus, 2003; Topbas, 2006; Höhle et al, 2009). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The semantic relationship between the verbs derived with {-lA} and the corresponding bases is not completely predictable: This, however, should not be taken to mean that morphological conversion is not available to Turkish as a word-formation process at all. In the next section, following Inkelas & Orgun (2003), I will present instances of word-formation, specifically derivation of toponyms out of nominals, which can be analyzed by a zero-derivation analysis.…”
Section: Noun/adjective-to-verb and Verb-to-noun/adjective Conversionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, this conclusion seems to be refuted by two other sets of data. Adopting Inkelas & Orgun's (2003) argument, I show that two sets of derived or non-derived words are converted into toponyms, and I argue that these two patterns can be claimed to involve zero affixes. The two zero affixes that are responsible for conversion are formally justified by the existence of their overt analogues.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…First of all, it would add additional machinery (see also Inkelas & Orgun 2003), as pre-stressing and stressed exceptional suffixes would be treated differently, despite the fact that there is good evidence for the two types of affixes to be treated in a unified manner, as both seem to be trochaic. Rather, their analysis predicts that there will be no stress, primary or secondary, after the first pre-stressing suffix.…”
Section: An Extraprosodicity Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%