2012
DOI: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01181.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Two‐ and Four‐Year‐Olds Learn to Adapt Referring Expressions to Context: Effects of Distracters and Feedback on Referential Communication

Abstract: Children often refer to things ambiguously but learn not to from responding to clarification requests. We review and explore this learning process here. In Study 1, eighty-four 2-and 4-year-olds were tested for their ability to request stickers from either (a) a small array with one dissimilar distracter or (b) a large array containing similar distracters. When children made ambiguous requests, they received either general feedback or specific questions about which of two options they wanted. With training, ch… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
79
1
9

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
(79 reference statements)
2
79
1
9
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, children as young as 3 years of age were more reluctant to imitate an adjective if they thought its use would be over-informative. This is in line with findings that, in simplified contexts, children as young as 2 years of age tend to avoid producing redundant information in their referring expressions (Matthews et al, 2007(Matthews et al, , 2012. Therefore it seems to be the case that from 3 years of age, children are able to make a simple association between the presence of a contrast set and the use of a modified expression (be this with an adjective or prepositional phrase).…”
Section: Comprehending Over-informative Utterancessupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Thus, children as young as 3 years of age were more reluctant to imitate an adjective if they thought its use would be over-informative. This is in line with findings that, in simplified contexts, children as young as 2 years of age tend to avoid producing redundant information in their referring expressions (Matthews et al, 2007(Matthews et al, , 2012. Therefore it seems to be the case that from 3 years of age, children are able to make a simple association between the presence of a contrast set and the use of a modified expression (be this with an adjective or prepositional phrase).…”
Section: Comprehending Over-informative Utterancessupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Olson, 1970;Pechmann, 1989;Brennan & Clark, 1996;Belke & Meyer, 2002), developmental psychologists (e.g. Ford & Olson, 1975;Deutsch & Pechmann, 1982;Matthews, Butcher, Lieven, & Tomasello, 2012), and computational linguists (e.g. Dale, 1989;Dale & Reiter, 1995;.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, they learn strategies that they have observed or learnt to be effective given the global goal (Matthews et al 2012). Whether this learning would generalize to novel narratives is an important outstanding question.…”
Section: Discussion: Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The models this provides are the active ingredients that help children learn. Children may have a global sense of the need to use these models without understanding their specific function at first (Matthews et al, 2012). Future research should explore the potentially powerful role for imitation when children have a clear goal and adopt adults' means of achieving it, at first without fully understanding these means (Klinger, Mayor & Bannard, 2016;Want & Harris, 2002;Whiten, McGuigan, Marshall-Pescini, & Hopper, 2009 A big boy and a little boy went to the park with their granddad.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation