2013
DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2013.860171
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Type-specific proactive interference in patients with semantic and phonological STM deficits

Abstract: Prior neuropsychological evidence suggests that semantic and phonological components of short-term memory (STM) are functionally and neurologically distinct. The current paper examines proactive interference (PI) from semantic and phonological information in two STM-impaired patients, DS (semantic STM deficit) and AK (phonological STM deficit). In Experiment 1 probe recognition tasks with open and closed sets of stimuli were used. Phonological PI was assessed using nonword items, and semantic and phonological … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, Harris and colleagues recently performed a memory rehabilitation study [26] with two older patients who, following a cerebral vascular accident, showed contrasting impairments in phonological STM versus semantic STM. Both interventions involved one 1.5 hour per session per week, over 10 weeks, in combination with at-home exercises.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, Harris and colleagues recently performed a memory rehabilitation study [26] with two older patients who, following a cerebral vascular accident, showed contrasting impairments in phonological STM versus semantic STM. Both interventions involved one 1.5 hour per session per week, over 10 weeks, in combination with at-home exercises.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite the fact that the treatment materials were confined to lists of words, the patient with impaired semantic STM showed significant improvements in sentence anomaly judgment following the semantic treatment intervention, but not after the phonologically-focused intervention, whereas the patient with impaired phonological STM showed post-treatment improvement on sentence repetition. Harris, Olson, and Humphreys [26] underscore the need for more research into STM rehabilitation, not only because of the high incidence of memory deficits following brain injury, but also because such interventions might lead to generalized treatment effects to other abilities (e.g., from word lists to sentences) and provide new insights into the processes supporting STM.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast to the inconsistent results for complex span measures and simple span measures tapping phonological storage, one measure which has been consistently related to argument integration during sentence processing is the category probe task, an index of semantic short-term memory (STM) in which participants are presented with a word list and asked to judge whether a probe word is in the same semantic category as any list word (Martin and Romani, 1994 ; Martin et al, 1994 ; Hanten and Martin, 2000 ; Martin and He, 2004 ; Martin, 2005 ; Harris et al, 2013 ). Martin et al ( 1994 ); Martin and He ( 2004 ) reported a double dissociation between aphasic patients with semantic STM deficits and patients with phonological STM deficits, with the two types of STM deficits having different consequences for sentence comprehension.…”
Section: Individual Differences In Wm and Their Role In Sentence Procmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, discrepancies between the findings of studies on interference may also arise from the various paradigms used to assess interference effects. Indeed, most studies comparing semantic and phonological interference have used working memory build-up of interference paradigms (Hamilton & Martin, 2007; Hanseeuw et al, 2010; Harris et al, 2014) or focused on generating false memories using Deese–Roediger–McDermott paradigms (Ballou & Sommers, 2008; Sommers & Huff, 2003; Sommers & Lewis, 1999; D. M. Wilson et al, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, previous studies obtained conflicting results concerning the impact of semantic knowledge breakdown on vulnerability to semantic interference. For example, some case studies of patients with isolated semantic or phonological deficits showed that the former was equally vulnerable to both types of interference due to global deficits in control processes (Hamilton & Martin, 2007) or even demonstrated heightened phonological interference and facilitated management of semantic interference (Harris et al, 2014). These results are also in line with those obtained by D.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%