2018
DOI: 10.1037/xge0000521
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Unconscious processing of facial dominance: The role of low-level factors in access to awareness.

Abstract: Visual stimuli with social-emotional relevance have been claimed to gain preferential access to awareness. For example, recent studies used the breaking continuous flash suppression paradigm (b-CFS) to show that faces that are perceived as less dominant and more trustworthy are prioritized in awareness. Here we asked whether these effects truly reflect differences in social-emotional meaning or whether they can be equally explained by differences in low-level stimulus properties. In Experiment 1, we successful… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
32
2

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
(86 reference statements)
1
32
2
Order By: Relevance
“…However, only a marginally significant difference in suppression times between the least trustworthy faces and neutral faces was found. Moreover, Stein et al (2018) replicated the findings of Stewart et al (2012) but suggested that the effect of facial dominance on suppression times was due to low-level physical stimulus characteristics. In addition, Abir et al (2018) found that more dominant faces and untrustworthy faces took shorter suppression times.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 61%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, only a marginally significant difference in suppression times between the least trustworthy faces and neutral faces was found. Moreover, Stein et al (2018) replicated the findings of Stewart et al (2012) but suggested that the effect of facial dominance on suppression times was due to low-level physical stimulus characteristics. In addition, Abir et al (2018) found that more dominant faces and untrustworthy faces took shorter suppression times.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…In addition, people needed only 40 ms to make judgments of dominance from faces (Rule et al, 2012). In recent years, a number of studies have applied the breaking continuous flash suppression paradigm (b-CFS; e.g., Jiang et al, 2007) to investigate the preconscious processing of faces, demonstrating that facial trustworthiness and dominance could still be processed preconsciously albeit inconsistently (Stewart et al, 2012; Getov et al, 2015; Abir et al, 2018; Stein et al, 2018). In b-CFS, noise patterns were presented to one of the participants’ eyes while faces were presented to the other eye.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our results are consistent with prior reports that would suggest so. Such features have been shown to drive the nonconscious processing of face identity ( Gelbard-Sagiv, Faivre, Mudrik, & Koch, 2016 ), expression ( Hedger et al, 2015 ), and dominance ( Stein, Awad, Gayet, & Peelen, 2018 ; Gayet et al, 2014 ), although it is yet unclear whether these features are themselves being processed nonconsciously ( Pitts, Martínez, & Hillyard, 2012 ), or if the effect is due to partial awareness ( Gelbard-Sagiv et al, 2016 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, shorter suppression times for fearful faces in CFS reflect higher effective contrast rather than enhanced processing of threat (Hedger, Adams, & Garner, 2015). Similarly, the influence of facial dominance on breaking CFS is related to local contrast in the face's eye regions rather than to social evaluation (Stein, Awad, Gayet, & Peelen, 2018). One elegant way of ruling out the influence of low-level stimulus properties is to use stimuli that differ only with regard to their affective learning history.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%