2016
DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1197195
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding body representations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
12
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
1
12
1
Order By: Relevance
“…4 ) of early stage potentials confirm the compatibility of early stage SEPs with a generator localized at the post-central gyrus (Broadmann areas 2 and 3) and are consistent with a physiological tactile activation of the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices 31 33 . Late Somatosensory Evoked Potentials (SEPs) are compatible with bilateral generators in the frontal lobes, including orbito-frontal, lateral and mesial cortex 34 , which confirms the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) support of bilateral tactile representation also seen in 33 , 35 . The compatibility of elicited SEPs with literature and the significant differences among stimulation modalities seem to confirm the prevalence of a main referred sensation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…4 ) of early stage potentials confirm the compatibility of early stage SEPs with a generator localized at the post-central gyrus (Broadmann areas 2 and 3) and are consistent with a physiological tactile activation of the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices 31 33 . Late Somatosensory Evoked Potentials (SEPs) are compatible with bilateral generators in the frontal lobes, including orbito-frontal, lateral and mesial cortex 34 , which confirms the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) support of bilateral tactile representation also seen in 33 , 35 . The compatibility of elicited SEPs with literature and the significant differences among stimulation modalities seem to confirm the prevalence of a main referred sensation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…The body is a unique multisensory object (Longo et al, 2008a) integrating a large variety of inputs both from the outside and from within the body (Gallace and Spence, 2008), thus offering the opportunity for a better interaction with the complex surrounding world (Medina and Coslett, 2016a). We can experience our own body through the basic somatic sensations of touch, warmth, cold, proprioception, nociception and itch coming from peripheral receptors to central specific cortical areas (somatosensation).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The achievement of this more sophisticated perceptual experience requires moving beyond pure somatosensation to a higher-order level of neural machinery in which a combination of somatic information converges in associative areas (Murata and Ishida, 2007;Murata et al, 2016) to produce a multimodal representation of the body as a whole (the so called body matrix) (Moseley et al, 2012). This "on line" organization of somatic information is checked for congruence against internal body models (somatorepresentations) (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005;de Vignemont, 2007;Carruthers, 2008;Tsakiris and Fotopoulou, 2008;Berlucchi and Aglioti, 2010;Longo, 2015;Medina and Coslett, 2016a): if the "on line" representation does not match (Azañón and Haggard, 2009) the "off line" body memory (Riva, 2018) we experience a body incoherence, from which misperceptions and bodily illusions may arise.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given the greater ability of representations in MPFC and pSTS to generalize across different stimuli (Skerry and Saxe [2014]) than representations in OFA and FFA, we interpret these influences as top-down. A recent study (Kliemann et al [2016]) has reported task-dependent changes in the patterns of response in ventral temporal face-selective regions. Participants were asked to judge the age (in half of the trials) and the valence (in the remaining trials) of facial expressions, and found that patterns of response in the right FFA were more similar in the trials in which participants performed the same task than in trials in which they performed different tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In each experiment, videos prompting the attribution of an emotion to a target were shown to participants while fMRI data were collected. The blood-oxygen level dependent (BOLD) timeseries measured were used to model connectivity between face selective regions and regions encoding information about emotional valence (OFA, FFA, pSTS and MPFC, see Skerry and Saxe [2014], Kliemann et al [2016]). …”
Section: Experimental Materials and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%