2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.10.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding the coherence of the severity effect and optimism phenomena: Lessons from attention

Abstract: Claims that optimism is a near-universal characteristic of human judgment seem to be at odds with recent results from the judgment and decision making literature suggesting that the likelihood of negative outcomes are overestimated relative to neutral outcomes. In an attempt to reconcile these seemingly contrasting phenomena, inspiration is drawn from the attention literature in which there is evidence that both positive and negative stimuli can have attentional privilege relative to neutral stimuli. This resu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 105 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Given the challenges in measuring and interpreting comparative optimism (e.g., Harris, ; Harris & Hahn, ), there are important considerations to note. First, comparative estimates may be egocentrically driven, resulting in comparative estimates that simply reflect personal judgements (Chambers et al ., ; Eiser et al ., ; Klar & Giladi, ; Kruger, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given the challenges in measuring and interpreting comparative optimism (e.g., Harris, ; Harris & Hahn, ), there are important considerations to note. First, comparative estimates may be egocentrically driven, resulting in comparative estimates that simply reflect personal judgements (Chambers et al ., ; Eiser et al ., ; Klar & Giladi, ; Kruger, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They are quite correct that the scale artifacts posited in [ 28 ] only directly challenge results obtained via the comparative method and thus the phenomenon of unrealistic comparative optimism at the group level (in the terminology of [ 34 ]). Our own review of the literature suggests that the evidence for other types of optimism (e.g., absolute optimism or, relatedly, the wishful thinking effect, whereby the desirability of an outcome causes an inflated probability estimate) is likewise overstated (see also, [ 21 , 28 , 41 , 61 , 63 , 69 , 70 ]). The current paper is not, however, the appropriate outlet for this debate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consequently, Studies 2–5 are closer in spirit to the ‘indirect’ methodology in comparative optimism studies, whereby participants provide separate estimates of their own chance and the average person’s chance, with bias inferred if there is a reliable difference between these estimates (see e.g., [ 29 , 34 ]). Whilst traditional studies using real-world events can be critiqued on similar grounds to the direct method [ 28 , 55 , 63 ], the present studies maintain tight control in providing participants with identical information across the two conditions. If traditional unrealistic optimism data reflect a genuine biasing effect of motivation on likelihood estimates, a difference in estimates should be observed between conditions in studies such as these.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 94%
“…More generally, the present findings have important implications for the debates about whether, how, and how strongly motivations bias expectations (Bar-Hillel et al, 2008a; Bilgin, 2012; Biner et al, 2009; de Molière & Harris, 2016; Harris, 2017; Harris et al, 2009; Harris & Corner, 2011; Lench, 2009; Logg et al, 2018; Massey et al, 2011; Simmons & Massey, 2012; Slovic, 1966). Terms like expectations and forecasts are broad ones.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%