2023
DOI: 10.2196/45482
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Using Normative Language When Describing Scientific Findings: Randomized Controlled Trial of Effects on Trust and Credibility

Abstract: Background Scientists often make cognitive claims (eg, the results of their work) and normative claims (eg, what should be done based on those results). Yet, these types of statements contain very different information and implications. This randomized controlled trial sought to characterize the granular effects of using normative language in science communication. Objective Our study examined whether viewing a social media post containing scientific cl… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
1

Relationship

1
0

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1 publication
(1 citation statement)
references
References 48 publications
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They also point to a funding environment that fosters “conservative projects with predictable results,” which may not include implementation effectiveness studies (Bauer et al, 2015). Further, there is a fundamental philosophical difference between knowing something is likely to be true of “intervention A” (e.g., intervention A works to reduce incidence of health issue B) and believing that intervention A should be implemented, or that it is in a given organization’s interests to do so (e.g., see Background of Agley et al (2023) for discussion of normative and cognitive claims around scientific findings). Multiple factors beyond efficacy are involved in decision-making about EBP implementation, such as cost, feasibility, and readiness – these questions of uptake are core elements of implementation science (Bauer & Kirchner, 2020).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They also point to a funding environment that fosters “conservative projects with predictable results,” which may not include implementation effectiveness studies (Bauer et al, 2015). Further, there is a fundamental philosophical difference between knowing something is likely to be true of “intervention A” (e.g., intervention A works to reduce incidence of health issue B) and believing that intervention A should be implemented, or that it is in a given organization’s interests to do so (e.g., see Background of Agley et al (2023) for discussion of normative and cognitive claims around scientific findings). Multiple factors beyond efficacy are involved in decision-making about EBP implementation, such as cost, feasibility, and readiness – these questions of uptake are core elements of implementation science (Bauer & Kirchner, 2020).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%