Dorrance, Kaiser, and Zentall (1998) Kaiser, and Zentall (1998) have reported a series of experiments on simultaneous discrimination learning by pigeons in which the value ofthe positive discriminative stimulus (S+) was varied outside the context of the simultaneous discrimination. Following concurrent training on two such discriminations, the relative.value of the two S-stimuli was assessed by means of a choice procedure. In all four experiments, an S-trained in a discrimination with an S+ of higher value was preferred. A simplified version of the design, denoted Design I, is shown in Table I. Each of two pairs of stimuli (A-B and C-D) is presented on a series of simultaneous discrimination trials, with choice responses to A or C reinforced, and choice responses to B or D, nonreinforced. Interspersed with these trials are single-stimulus trials on which stimulus A or C is presented alone, with responding to stimulus A reinforced with a greater probability than that for responding to stimulus C. Following this training, the pigeons were presented with a choice between Band D. In each of the four experiments, the pigeons showed a preference for stimulus B. Dorrance et al. (1998) interpret these results in terms of value transfer theory (VTT), in which it is assumed that some ofthe value ofthe S+ in a simultaneous discrimination transfers to the S-. Thus, according to VTT, choice responding would be more likely to B than to D in the simplified design shown in Table I, because more value would transfer to B from A, which is reinforced during the single-stimulus trials, than would transfer from C to D. The authors compare this account with an occasion setting account, which fails, as the authors note, to predict the observed preferences in any of the four experiments.Value transfer is not the only mechanism that can account for these data, however. The greater reinforcement of stimulus A over stimulus C on the interspersed trials would presumably cause the two simultaneous discriminations in Design I to differ in difficulty. As a result, more errors would be observed on C-D trials than on A-B trials, possibly leading to a greater degree of inhibitory control accruing to stimulus D. Such a difference was clear in Dorrance et al.'s (1998) Experiments 1,2, and 4, and it was precisely these experiments that revealed the greatest preference of B over D. Dorrance et al. seek to discount this simple mechanism, based on the different reinforcement history of stimuli Band D, by means of a simple, yet problematic, analysis. They report a negative correlation in three out of the four experiments (Experiments 1,2, and 3) between two measures: the proportion ofnonreinforced responses to Band D that were to D during training (%D choice), and the proportion of test responses on B-D trials that were to B (%B choice). They argue that this contradicts the simpler account: If the preference for B over D depended on the birds' having made more nonreinforced responses to D, one would expect a positive correlation between these two meas...