2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2014.10.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variation in the prosody of focus in head- and head/edge-prominence languages

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
28
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
2
28
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This strategy was most frequently found in L2s' speech. As post-focal deaccenting is a strategy that is commonly used in English (Burdin et al, 2015;Ito & Speer, 2008), verb deaccenting after a focused subject found in the L2 data is likely to be due to influence from English. The HSs also used this cue, but to a lesser degree.…”
Section: Variability In Focus Marking Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This strategy was most frequently found in L2s' speech. As post-focal deaccenting is a strategy that is commonly used in English (Burdin et al, 2015;Ito & Speer, 2008), verb deaccenting after a focused subject found in the L2 data is likely to be due to influence from English. The HSs also used this cue, but to a lesser degree.…”
Section: Variability In Focus Marking Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Spanish and English differ particularly in pitch contour. In English, words in a non-final position of a declarative sentence usually bear a high pitch accent (H*), but when these words are focused, they are generally expressed with a rising pitch accent (L+H*) (Beckman, Hirschberg, & Shattuck-Hufnagel, 2005) and the following non-focused words tend to be deaccented, lending prosodic prominence to the focused words (Burdin et al, 2015;Ito & Speer, 2008). In Spanish, while there is a large variation in pitch accent types (Kim & Avelino, 2003;Martín Butragueño, 2005, 2006, non-focused words in this position are usually produced with a rising pitch movement that continues throughout the stressed syllable until the syllable(s) that follow(s) (L+>H*) (Estebas-Vilaplana, 2007;Prieto, van Santen, & Hirschberg, 1995).…”
Section: Focus In Spanish and Englishmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similar patterns are reported for K'iche' in Can Pixabaj & England (), but cf. Yasavul (), Burdin et al (). Velleman () discusses interactions between in situ focus and prosody in Kichee', arguing that prosody cannot be the sole determiner of focus for post‐verbal subjects; see also Henderson ().…”
Section: Phrasal Prosody: the Word And Abovementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dedicated studies of intonation in Mayan languages include Larsen & Pike (1949), Berinstein (1991), Nielsen (2005), Shklovsky (2011), Burdin et al (2015), among other work already cited above. Pye (1983Pye ( , 1992 argues that final stress and intonation play an important role in language acquisition in K'iche'.…”
Section: Phrasal Prosody: the Word And Above 71 Intonation And Phramentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The notation is also useful in clarifying points of typological variation. Some languages appear not to mark focus prosodically, like certain African languages (Güldemann et al, 2015), or the Mayan language K'iche' (Burdin et al, 2015). Do these lack ∼ altogether, or do they just lack the phonological means to encode the scope of ∼?…”
Section: Using Rooth's Notation As a "Normal Form"mentioning
confidence: 99%