2013
DOI: 10.1002/mcda.1500
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variation of Preference Inconsistency When Applying Ratio and Interval Scale Pairwise Comparisons

Abstract: Several studies on numerical rating in discrete choice problems address the tendency of inconsistencies in decision makers' measured preferences. This is partly due to true inconsistencies in preferences or the decision makers' uncertainty on what he or she really wants. This uncertainty may be reflected in the elicited preferences in different ways depending on the questions asked and methods used in deriving the preferences for alternatives. Some part of the inconsistency is due to only having a discrete set… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…According to our tests, even a small inconsistency in just one of the comparisons could increase the CR above the threshold of 0.1. Similar problems are reported in ratio-scale pairwise comparisons based on other types of surveys (Sironen et al 2013), and other measures have been presented for evaluating the inconsistencies (Peláez and Lamata 2003;Ramík and Korviny 2010) and uncertainties related to the process overall (see Alho and Kangas 1997), differing from those originally proposed by Saaty (1980). According to the earlier studies on the preferences on forest use, the majority of citizens, regardless of forest ownership, do not approve of clearcutting (Valkeapää and Karppinen 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…According to our tests, even a small inconsistency in just one of the comparisons could increase the CR above the threshold of 0.1. Similar problems are reported in ratio-scale pairwise comparisons based on other types of surveys (Sironen et al 2013), and other measures have been presented for evaluating the inconsistencies (Peláez and Lamata 2003;Ramík and Korviny 2010) and uncertainties related to the process overall (see Alho and Kangas 1997), differing from those originally proposed by Saaty (1980). According to the earlier studies on the preferences on forest use, the majority of citizens, regardless of forest ownership, do not approve of clearcutting (Valkeapää and Karppinen 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…Management of inconsistent preferences has received limited attention in the reviewed studies. It is though well known that the same DM might be inconsistent in his/her judgments, or the group providing the preferences may not unanimously agree with the same preferences [ 48 , 155 ]. As a consequence, it is desirable to have more MCDA methods that can deal with these types of decision context due to their inbuilt features.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In terms of spatial cognition other measures of spatial knowledge, learning, and familiarity could have been included in our survey (Golledge 1993;Tu Huynh and Doherty 2007). Likewise, the use of Likert-scale levels, specific preference elicitation methods, and the time of survey implementation (December-January is a holiday season in Colombia) can have an influence on response rates and the statistical results (Sironen et al 2014). Finally, such response rates can be an indication of not only survey exhaustion or protest responses, but also respondent's lack of spatial knowledge and abilities to read a satellite image and maps.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%