1986
DOI: 10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.575
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variations in Stress Responses as a Function of Cognitive and Personality Variables

Abstract: Prior research shows wide individual differences in perception of and response to stressful life events. The present study examined the extent to which those differences could be attributable to individual differences in one's feelings of general self-efficacy or to characteristic ways of attributing causality for possible successful resolution of the problem posed. A sample of 273 undergraduate students were surveyed to ascertain their estimates of the stressfulness of four of 16 stressful life events as well… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1996
1996
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In their theory of learned helplessness, Seligman and his colleagues (e.g., Abramson et al, 1978;Peterson & Seligman, 1984;Seligman & Elder, 1986) have examined explanatory style as a risk factor for depression. According to the theory, depression is more likely to occur in individuals who routinely make internal, stable, and global (i.e., generalizations of causal explanations over situations) attributions for failure; and recent research is beginning to show that this causal belief is relevant to achievement events as well (Anderson, 1983;Compas, Forsythe, & Wagner, 1988;Follette & Jacobson, 1987;Metalsky et al, 1982;Morgan, Owen, Miller, & Watts, 1986;Peterson & Barrett, 1987;Peterson, Colvin, & Lin, 1992;Petiprin & Johnson, 1991).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In their theory of learned helplessness, Seligman and his colleagues (e.g., Abramson et al, 1978;Peterson & Seligman, 1984;Seligman & Elder, 1986) have examined explanatory style as a risk factor for depression. According to the theory, depression is more likely to occur in individuals who routinely make internal, stable, and global (i.e., generalizations of causal explanations over situations) attributions for failure; and recent research is beginning to show that this causal belief is relevant to achievement events as well (Anderson, 1983;Compas, Forsythe, & Wagner, 1988;Follette & Jacobson, 1987;Metalsky et al, 1982;Morgan, Owen, Miller, & Watts, 1986;Peterson & Barrett, 1987;Peterson, Colvin, & Lin, 1992;Petiprin & Johnson, 1991).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Those dimensions include hardiness (Florian, Mikulincer, & Taubman, 1995;Kobasa, 1979;Rhodewalt and Agustdottir, 1984;Rhodewalt & Zone, 1989;Wiebe & Williams, 1992); Type A personality (Smith & Rhodewalt, 1986); locus of control (Anderson, 1977;Fame, Sebellico, Gnugnoli, & Coralio, 1992;Jorgensen & Johnson, 1990;Parkes, 1984;Vitaliano, Russo, & Maiuro, 1987); general self-efficacy, helplessness (Jerusalem, 1992;Morgan, Owen, Miller, & Watts, 1986); traits less relevant for control such as sensation seeking (Horvath & Zuckerman, 1992); trait negative/positive affectivity (Elliot, Chartrand & Harkins, 1994); insecurity (Martin & Lee, 1992); depression-prone personality types (Mongrain & Zuroff, 1989); and extraversion and neuroticism (Gallagher, 1990). We conceptualize a dispositional appraisal dimension related to, but not synonymous with, these dimensions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although responses to stressful situations are relatively stable over time (Andreasen, 1984;Tice, 1992), not everyone who experiences an event perceives it as equally stressful (Morgan, Owen, Miller, & Watts, 1986). In general, however, undesirable events are more likely to lead to elevated levels of stress than are either desirable or ambiguous events (Zautra, Guarnaccia, Reich, Bruce, & Dohrenwend, 1986).…”
Section: Life Events and Stressmentioning
confidence: 98%