2021
DOI: 10.1017/langcog.2021.8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verb argument structure overgeneralisations for the English intransitive and transitive constructions: grammaticality judgments and production priming

Abstract: We used a multi-method approach to investigate how children avoid (or retreat from) argument structure overgeneralisation errors (e.g., *You giggled me). Experiment 1 investigated how semantic and statistical constraints (preemption and entrenchment) influence children’s and adults’ judgments of the grammatical acceptability of 120 verbs in transitive and intransitive sentences. Experiment 2 used syntactic priming to elicit overgeneralisation errors from children (aged 5–6) to investigate whether the same cons… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One possibility is that the “retreat from overgeneralization” is largely accomplished by age 4–5; the youngest age-group in the present study. However, this does not seem likely, given that the relevant errors are attested amongst children aged 4 years and above in (a) the present study, (b) previous experimental studies ( Bidgood et al , 2021 ; Fukuda & Fukuda, 2001 ), and (c) diary data ( Ambridge & Ambridge, 2020 ; Bowerman, 1988 ; Nakaishi, 2016 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…One possibility is that the “retreat from overgeneralization” is largely accomplished by age 4–5; the youngest age-group in the present study. However, this does not seem likely, given that the relevant errors are attested amongst children aged 4 years and above in (a) the present study, (b) previous experimental studies ( Bidgood et al , 2021 ; Fukuda & Fukuda, 2001 ), and (c) diary data ( Ambridge & Ambridge, 2020 ; Bowerman, 1988 ; Nakaishi, 2016 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…In contrast, *the clown laughed the man is not a possible utterance because the causing event (e.g., the clown telling a joke) and the caused event (i.e., the man laughing) are two events that are relatively distinct in time and space. For evidence of the importance of meaning in restricting these types of generalizations see—for the transitive causative—Pinker (1989), Brooks and Tomasello (1999), Ambridge et al (2008), Ambridge et al (2009), Ambridge et al (2011), Ambridge et al (2020), Ambridge et al (2022), Bidgood et al (2021) and—for other constructions—Ambridge et al (2012), Ambridge (2013), Ambridge et al (2014), Bidgood et al (2014), Blything et al (2014), Ambridge and Blything (2016). However, semantics alone is not a complete solution, since some restrictions seem to be semantically arbitrary (e.g., manage to do; succeed in doing vs. *succeed to do ; *manage in doing ).…”
Section: Argument Structure Overgeneralization Errorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This even involves arguments of the predicate, or traditionally termed as objects (Noble, Rowland, & Pine, 2011, p. 964). The number of arguments taken by a predicate determines the type of the predicate, i.e., one-place predicate (intransitive), two-place predicate (transitive), or three-place predicate (ditransitive) (Bidgood et al, 2021;Shin, 2017, p. 12). A representation of the number and semantic roles of arguments of a predicate is called argument structure (Akbarnezhad, Sadighi, & Bagheri, 2020).…”
Section: Verb Transitivitymentioning
confidence: 99%