2017
DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2017.1285238
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verb Learning in 14- and 18-Month-Old English-Learning Infants

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
74
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
1
74
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, children may exploit the fact that an unknown word occurs in a noun context to infer that it probably refers to an object, while words occurring in verb contexts probably refer to actions (e.g., Gillette et al, 1999;Gleitman, 1990). For instance, He and Lidz (2014) showed that 18-month-olds (but not 14-month-olds) were able to infer that a novel word such as 'doke' referred to an object when listening to sentences such as "Look, it's a doke! ", and that a novel word such as 'pratch' referred to an action when listening to sentences such as "Look!…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, children may exploit the fact that an unknown word occurs in a noun context to infer that it probably refers to an object, while words occurring in verb contexts probably refer to actions (e.g., Gillette et al, 1999;Gleitman, 1990). For instance, He and Lidz (2014) showed that 18-month-olds (but not 14-month-olds) were able to infer that a novel word such as 'doke' referred to an object when listening to sentences such as "Look, it's a doke! ", and that a novel word such as 'pratch' referred to an action when listening to sentences such as "Look!…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…But modified subjects (e.g., ‘the nice tall boy’) pose too high of a processing demand for 2‐ and 3‐year‐olds . For younger children, at age 22 months, even a single content noun may pose too great a processing demand; He and Lidz found that in a simpler learning situation children performed better with ‘it’ or ‘that thing’ in subject position of a novel verb than the more informative but more difficult‐to‐process ‘the balloon.’ For older children, age 5 years, pronouns are sufficient, but contexts with no overt arguments (e.g., ‘Pilking!’) are not informative enough . Similarly, in a different paradigm, pilot studies suggest that novel nouns replacing the content nouns (e.g., ‘The dax is gonna pilk the blick’) may not support 2‐year‐olds’ acquisition of a novel verb, but may suffice for 3.5‐year‐olds .…”
Section: Identifying the Conceptmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…By 18 months of age, infants infer whether a novel word is a noun or verb, based on phrasal prosody (e.g., [la petite bamoule ] (the small bamoule ) vs. ‘[la petite] [ bamoule ]’ (the small (one) bamoules ), with ‘[]’ indicating phrasal prosodic boundaries) . Once a novel word's grammatical category is identified, the learner can narrow down their hypotheses about its meaning by utilizing relationships between grammatical and conceptual categories—for example, nouns typically name object kinds and verbs name event categories …”
Section: Finding the Wordsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Infants start to learn word and referent associations as early as 6–8 months (e.g., Tincoff & Jusczyk, ) and produce words by the end of their first year. A wide range of studies has suggested that word‐referent learning is influenced by multiple factors such as the learner's current level of vocabulary development (Werker, Fennell, Corcoran, & Stager, ) and language background (Burnham et al., ; Fennell, Byers‐Heinlein, & Werker, ; Singh, ; Singh et al., , ), the familiarity of the word being learned (Fennell & Werker, ), and the presence or absence of synchrony between word presentation and object motion during the task (Gogate, ; see also Gogate & Maganti, ; He & Lidz, for verb‐action learning). Phonetic properties are also known to be influential in word‐referent, especially word–object, learning.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%