1973
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1973.37.2.531
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Verbal Reinforcement Combinations, Task Complexity, Age and Sex in Children's Discrimination Learning

Abstract: 144 boys and girls in Grades 2 and 6 were presented a 2-choice simultaneous brightness-discrimination task. Ss were either verbally reinforced for correct responses (Rn), verbally punished for incorrect responses (Wn), or verbally reinforced for correct and punished for incorrect responses (RW). Two levels of task complexity defined as the number of irrelevant stimulus dimensions were utilized. Regardless of Ss' sex, age, or the complexity of the task, the Rn combination produced significantly slower learning … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1994
1994
1998
1998

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Programmed instruction has been conceptualized as an elaborate form of discrimination training (Skinner, 1968), and punishment of incorrect responses is one of the most effective ways to improve discrimination performance (Getsie, Langer, & Glass, 1985). This has been shown consistently with laboratory animals (Fowler, Hochhauser, & Wischner, 1981), intellectually disabled children (Harris & Tramontana, 1973), intellectually normal children (Miller, Moffat, Cotter, & Ochocki, 1973), impulsive children (Hemry, 1973), hyperactive children (Cunningham & Knights, 1978), and adults (Matthews & Shimoff, 1974). The functions of punishment most useful in learning are its ability to decrease error rates, slow an organism's rate of responding (Borresen, 1973;Donahue & Ratliff, 1976;Tindall & Ratliff, 1974), and get the organism to attend carefully to stimulus features in the environment (Balaban, Rhodes, & Neuringer, 1990;Muenzinger, 1934;Ratliff & Root, 1974).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Programmed instruction has been conceptualized as an elaborate form of discrimination training (Skinner, 1968), and punishment of incorrect responses is one of the most effective ways to improve discrimination performance (Getsie, Langer, & Glass, 1985). This has been shown consistently with laboratory animals (Fowler, Hochhauser, & Wischner, 1981), intellectually disabled children (Harris & Tramontana, 1973), intellectually normal children (Miller, Moffat, Cotter, & Ochocki, 1973), impulsive children (Hemry, 1973), hyperactive children (Cunningham & Knights, 1978), and adults (Matthews & Shimoff, 1974). The functions of punishment most useful in learning are its ability to decrease error rates, slow an organism's rate of responding (Borresen, 1973;Donahue & Ratliff, 1976;Tindall & Ratliff, 1974), and get the organism to attend carefully to stimulus features in the environment (Balaban, Rhodes, & Neuringer, 1990;Muenzinger, 1934;Ratliff & Root, 1974).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Delays may also have been aversive. When punishment is employed in discrimination training, subjects respond more slowly and attend more carefully to stimulus features (Barlow, 1933;Muenzinger, 1934aMuenzinger, , 1934b, and discriminated performance improves (Getsie, Langer, & Glass, 1985;Harris & Tramontana, 1973;F. Miller, Moffat, Cotter, & Ochocki, 1973).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A similar problem exists for studies examining reinforcement and punishment during children's learning where verbal praise and/or reprimands were provided as a consequence for response accuracy (Firestone & Douglas, 1977;Foxx, 1984;Hemry, 1973;Moses et al, 1979;Miller et al, 1973;Mueller & Gumina, 1972). The functional value of a consequence such as praise or reprimands may vary widely among individuals, as was demonstrated by Costello and Ferrer (1976).…”
Section: Assessment Of Functional Consequencesmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…During learning tasks, complex stimuli were often used that varied across a number of possibly relevant dimensions, including color, shape, position, and size. Establishing discriminative control of a subject's selection by isolated properties of the stimuli was invariably most difficult to accomplish using reinforcement alone; punishment in isolation or in combination with reinforcement was the most effective consequence for bringing the choice behavior of human subjects under the control of the relevant properties of the learning task (Brackbill & O'Hara, 1958;Donahue & Ratliff, 1976;Harris & Tramontana, 1973;Hemry, 1973;Miller, Moffat, Cotter, & Ochocki, 1973;Mueller & Gumina, 1972;Ochocki, Cotter, & Miller, 1974;Penney, 1967;Ratliff & Root, 1974;Stevenson, Weir, & Zigler, 1959;Tindall & Ratliff, 1974;Trent, 1983;Witte & Johnson, 1973). These results were replicated across special populations of children, including hyperactive boys (Cunningham & Knights, 1978), impulsive children (Firestone & Douglas, 1977), and delinquent boys (Moses, Ratliff, & Ratliff, 1979).…”
Section: Simple Discrimination Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation