2005
DOI: 10.1080/13552600500063666
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Viewing time measures and sexual interest: another piece of the puzzle

Abstract: The present study compared a viewing time (VT) measure with the Sexual Deviance Card Sort (Laws et al., 2000) and past sexual behaviour. Twenty-six adult males who committed a contact sexual offence (19 of whom had child victims and seven with adult victims) each completed the self-report card sort and viewed 640 slides of nude and clothed males and females, ages 5, 9, 13 years and adult. The offenders were unaware that their viewing time was being recorded. VT allowed for greater consistent classification of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
38
0
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
2
38
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This basic finding by Rosenzweig (1942) has been systematically exploited in applied settings to infer sexual preferences from response latencies. Importantly, these latencies do not only differ reliably between individuals with varying sexual orientation (i.e., hetero-vs. homosexual men; Imhoff et al, 2010;Israel & Strassberg, 2007;Quinsey, Ketsetzis, Earls, & Karamanoukian, 1996;Zamansky, 1956), but also produce specific patterns for individuals with differing sexual age preferences (e.g., individuals with sexual interest in adults vs. children) (Abel, 1995;Abel, Huffman, Warberg, & Holland, 1998;Abel, Jordan, Hand, Holland, & Phipps, 2001;Banse, Schmidt, & Clarbour, 2010;Glasgow, Osborne, & Croxen, 2003;Gress, 2005;Harris, Rice, Quinsey, & Chaplin, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This basic finding by Rosenzweig (1942) has been systematically exploited in applied settings to infer sexual preferences from response latencies. Importantly, these latencies do not only differ reliably between individuals with varying sexual orientation (i.e., hetero-vs. homosexual men; Imhoff et al, 2010;Israel & Strassberg, 2007;Quinsey, Ketsetzis, Earls, & Karamanoukian, 1996;Zamansky, 1956), but also produce specific patterns for individuals with differing sexual age preferences (e.g., individuals with sexual interest in adults vs. children) (Abel, 1995;Abel, Huffman, Warberg, & Holland, 1998;Abel, Jordan, Hand, Holland, & Phipps, 2001;Banse, Schmidt, & Clarbour, 2010;Glasgow, Osborne, & Croxen, 2003;Gress, 2005;Harris, Rice, Quinsey, & Chaplin, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The response latency of this judgment is unobtrusively measured. Across studies, there is a very robust finding that the response latency is longer for sexually attractive as compared to sexually unattractive targets and, in turn, viewing time measures can be used to discriminate between participants with respect to sexual preference (Flak, Beech, & Fisher, 2007;Kalmus & Beech, 2005;Laws & Gress, 2004), including homosexual and heterosexual men (Zamansky, 1956), heterosexual men and women (Israel & Strassberg, 2009;Quinsey, Ketsetzis, Earls, & Karamanoukian, 1996), and child sex offenders and non-offenders (e.g., Banse, Schmidt, & Clarbour, in press;Gress, 2005;Harris et al, 1996).…”
Section: Viewing Time Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Viewing time (Abel, et al, 2004;Glasgow, Osborne, & Croxen, 2003;Gress, 2005;Harris, Rice, Quinsey, & Chaplin, 1996) asks participants to rate an image on some dimension (typically how attractive they find the individual depicted) while recording the time it takes to respond. This task, therefore, includes an explicit selfreport component (i.e.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%