2012
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1116141109
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual attention and the acquisition of information in human crowds

Abstract: Pedestrian crowds can form the substrate of important socially contagious behaviors, including propagation of visual attention, violence, opinions, and emotional state. However, relating individual to collective behavior is often difficult, and quantitative studies have largely used laboratory experimentation. We present two studies in which we tracked the motion and head direction of 3,325 pedestrians in natural crowds to quantify the extent, influence, and context dependence of socially transmitted visual at… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

13
232
6

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 206 publications
(251 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
13
232
6
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, many previous approaches derive from the form P x = f ðxÞ=ðf ðxÞ + f ðyÞÞ (21,22,27,28), which is also a particular case of P x = f ðx; yÞ=ðf ðx; yÞ + f ðy; xÞÞ, and therefore compatible with probability matching. In other cases, the basic form P x = f ðxÞ=ðf ðxÞ + f ðyÞÞ has been modified by adding constant terms (29,30) or an extra function (25), as P x = f ðxÞ=ðf ðxÞ + f ðkÞÞ, with k a constant when animals have access to a single choice (31,32,34). Weber behavior can also be seen as a particular case.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Interestingly, many previous approaches derive from the form P x = f ðxÞ=ðf ðxÞ + f ðyÞÞ (21,22,27,28), which is also a particular case of P x = f ðx; yÞ=ðf ðx; yÞ + f ðy; xÞÞ, and therefore compatible with probability matching. In other cases, the basic form P x = f ðxÞ=ðf ðxÞ + f ðyÞÞ has been modified by adding constant terms (29,30) or an extra function (25), as P x = f ðxÞ=ðf ðxÞ + f ðkÞÞ, with k a constant when animals have access to a single choice (31,32,34). Weber behavior can also be seen as a particular case.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Animals of different species have been observed to incorporate this extra information in their decisions in different ways. Some species make decisions that can be explained using the differences of the numbers of animals taking each option (21,22), others according to the relative differences (Weber's law) (23,24) or using other rules (25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33)(34). This diversity of decision schemes has translated into a diversity of models (21,22,(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33)(34).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This exploration meaningfully extends existing research by measuring interactional outcomes that are nonobservable to individual social actors. Research on information propagation (22), imitation (24), and social influence (25) study large-scale dynamics by focusing on behavior manifested in a social context (e.g., observed, interpreted, and acted on); however, this excludes a large class of situations that involve unobservable cognitive consequences of social interactions, such as mnemonic reinforcement and retrieval-induced forgetting effects. In this study, we fill this gap and provide a framework for investigating the emergent properties of individual cognition at a social level.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In humans or animals, collective behaviors are suggested to increase sensory range (Couzin, 2009), and may serve visual attention. However collective behavior can prove difficult to relate to individual behavior (Gallup et al, 2012). Here is further food for thoughts coming from the study of brain networks dynamics.…”
Section: Synchrony Is a Process And A Solutionmentioning
confidence: 99%