1997
DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

VISUAL ATTENTION: Control, Representation, and Time Course

Abstract: Three central problems in the recent literature on visual attention are reviewed. The first concerns the control of attention by top-down (or goal-directed) and bottom-up (or stimulus-driven) processes. The second concerns the representational basis for visual selection, including how much attention can be said to be location- or object-based. Finally, we consider the time course of attention as it is directed to one stimulus after another.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

31
731
2
17

Year Published

2002
2002
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,138 publications
(794 citation statements)
references
References 146 publications
31
731
2
17
Order By: Relevance
“…The demonstration that attention is directed to objects in space has since been con®rmed by many studies (reviewed in Ref. [16]). As a matter of fact, normal observers ®nd it extremely dif®cult, if not impossible, to covertly attend to à blank' region of space, where no object is present (see Experiment 2 in Ref.…”
Section: Orienting Of Attention To Visual Objectsmentioning
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The demonstration that attention is directed to objects in space has since been con®rmed by many studies (reviewed in Ref. [16]). As a matter of fact, normal observers ®nd it extremely dif®cult, if not impossible, to covertly attend to à blank' region of space, where no object is present (see Experiment 2 in Ref.…”
Section: Orienting Of Attention To Visual Objectsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…More recently, this distinction has been variously referred at as re¯exive/voluntary, bottom-up/ top-down, stimulus-driven/goal-directed or strategy-based, or exogenous/endogenous (see Ref. [16] for review). It is important to note that, logically speaking this dichotomy must be relative rather than absolute.…”
Section: The Exogenous/endogenous Dichotomymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An object may become the focus of attention due to its particular physical properties such as brightness or unique shape (bottom-up signals), or because it matches current preferences and action goals of the observer (top-down signals; Egeth and Yantis, 1997;Fecteau and Munoz, 2006;Pashler et al, 2001). A central question for the study of attention is whether a salient stimulus may capture attention automatically by overriding observer's goals or intentions (Itti and Koch, 2000;Theeuwes, 2004;Van der Stigchel et al, 2009;Yantis and Jonides, 1984), or whether attention selects stimuli based on their relevance and compatibility with current action goals (Folk et al, 1992;Gibson and Jiang, 1998;Simons, 2000).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, even if acquisition of useful linguistic information from a parafoveal target preview required the allocation of attention to it, it is not clear that such a shift of attention was delayed relative to the onset of a fixation. Estimates of the minimal time needed for an attention shift from one location to another are in the vicinity of 50 ms (Posner, 1980;Treisman & Gelade, 1980;Wolfe, 1998), although there are other persuasive views that propose that it may take considerably longer than that (see Egeth & Yantis, 1997;Horowitz, Holcombe, Wolfe, Arsenio, & DiMase, 2004;Theeuwes, Godijn, & Pratt, 2004;Ward, 2001). For example, Horowitz et al (2004) suggest that "attentional saccades" between objects may take as much as 300 ms.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%