“…The following examples represent typical measurement of social attention in the ASD literature:
- duration of looking to people (face, eyes, mouth) while viewing photographs (Birmingham, Cerf, & Adolphs, 2011; Sasson & Touchstone, 2013), movies (Chawarska, Macari, & Shic, 2012; Parish-Morris et al, 2013), or during live interaction (Freeth, Foulsham, & Kingstone, 2013; Hutman, Chela, Gillespie-Lynch, & Sigman, 2012);
- orienting (e.g., turning head and/or eyes) to people (Laidlaw, Foulsham, Kuhn, & Kingstone, 2011; Maestro et al, 2002, 2005) or human sounds (Dawson, Toth, et al, 2004); change detection across two nearly identical social scenes (New et al, 2010);
- gaze following/attention cueing (Greene et al, 2011; Riby, Hancock, Jones, & Hanley, 2013);
- attention shifting between people and objects (Hutman et al);
- joint attention behaviors including responding to (e.g., turning eyes and/or head to follow examiner’s point and gaze) and initiating (e.g., gaze, alternating gaze, showing, pointing to share attention) coordinated attention with others (e.g., Barbaro & Dissanayake, 2010; Bedford et al, 2014); smiling and vocalizing while interacting with others (e.g., Maestro et al).
The wide variability in conceptualization and measurement of social attention in ASD is clearly apparent in these examples (note that use of the term has been aligned with all three functional categories). In each case, the stated (or implicit) assumption is that various indices of attention to people and/or social communication behaviors (joint attention) operate as a proxy for indexing clinically relevant social attention differences in ASD.…”