1981
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.7.4.795
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual information about moving objects.

Abstract: A mathematical model of visual flow is presented, which could potentially account for an observer's ability to effectively interact with moving objects. The analysis demonstrates that there is visual information available to an observer about (a) a moving object's angle of approach, (b) changes in its velocity and acceleration, (c) whether its velocity and acceleration are positive or negative, (d) its time to collision with both the horizontal and vertical axes, and (e) whether it will cross the horizontal ax… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

13
251
4

Year Published

1990
1990
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 280 publications
(268 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
13
251
4
Order By: Relevance
“…In all such studies, however, information has been conceived in its traditional interpretation of the amount of uncertainty (Shannon & Weaver, 1949) rather than in terms of a lawful specification of events (Gibson, 1979) and invoked, moreover, an arbitrary coupling between stimulus and response. Having to increase or reverse the speed of movement on the basis of some light coming on at some unpredictable moment during movement execution, as in the Quinn and Sherwood (1983) study, can hardly be compared with, let alone generalized to, movement execution on the basis of a continuous information flow specifying present and future events (Bootsma, 1989;Fitch & Turvey, 1978;Kugler, Turvey, Carello, & Shaw, 1985;Lee, Young, Reddish, Lough, & Clayton, 1983;Runeson & Frykholm, 1983;Todd, 1981;Turvey & CareUo, 1986;Warren, Young, & Lee, 1986). Experiments in which subjects operate under real-life conditions can be logically expected to render a more thorough insight into the way behavior is coordinated with events in the environment.…”
Section: Continuous Guidancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In all such studies, however, information has been conceived in its traditional interpretation of the amount of uncertainty (Shannon & Weaver, 1949) rather than in terms of a lawful specification of events (Gibson, 1979) and invoked, moreover, an arbitrary coupling between stimulus and response. Having to increase or reverse the speed of movement on the basis of some light coming on at some unpredictable moment during movement execution, as in the Quinn and Sherwood (1983) study, can hardly be compared with, let alone generalized to, movement execution on the basis of a continuous information flow specifying present and future events (Bootsma, 1989;Fitch & Turvey, 1978;Kugler, Turvey, Carello, & Shaw, 1985;Lee, Young, Reddish, Lough, & Clayton, 1983;Runeson & Frykholm, 1983;Todd, 1981;Turvey & CareUo, 1986;Warren, Young, & Lee, 1986). Experiments in which subjects operate under real-life conditions can be logically expected to render a more thorough insight into the way behavior is coordinated with events in the environment.…”
Section: Continuous Guidancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Using the same type of analysis as Todd (1981), an optical variable specifying future passing distance can be derived (cf. Bootsma, 1991;Bootsma & Peper, 1992).…”
Section: Optical Specification Of Passing Distancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The dot notation is used to indicate the time derivatives: Velocity is denoted by a single dot, acceleration by two dots. Because optical variables associated with one surface can be uniquely transformed into optical variables associated with any other surface (Lee, 1974;Todd, 1981), the present analysis is performed in reference to a planar surface parallel to the approaching object at unit distance from the point of observation, for reasons of mathematical convenience (Figure 1).…”
Section: Optical Specification Of Passing Distancementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…τ is defined as the ratio of an object's instantaneous optical size to its instantaneous rate of optical expansion (Lee, 1976). Observers use or are sensitive to τ in a variety of tasks such as absolute and relative TTC judgments (e.g., (e.g., Bootsma & Oudejans, 1993;Schiff & Oldak, 1990;Todd, 1981). τ does not require perceptual estimates of the incoming object's absolute size, distance, or velocity.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%