2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.tine.2016.07.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual-motor functional connectivity in preschool children emerges after handwriting experience

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
52
0
2

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(55 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
1
52
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…These findings resonate with other evidence that visual production can support learning, including mainte-nance of recently learned information (Wammes et al, 2016) and enhanced recognition of novel symbols (Longcamp et al, 2008;James and Atwood, 2009;Li and James, 2016). Previous fMRI studies that have investigated the neural mechanisms underlying such learning have found enhanced activation in visual cortex when viewing previously practiced letters (James and Gauthier, 2006;James, 2017), and increased connectivity between visual and parietal regions following handwriting experience (Vinci-Booher et al, 2016). However, these studies have focused on univariate measures of BOLD signal amplitude within regions or when analyzing connectivity, raising the question of whether these changes reflect the recruitment of similar representations across tasks or of colocated but functionally distinct representations for each task.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…These findings resonate with other evidence that visual production can support learning, including mainte-nance of recently learned information (Wammes et al, 2016) and enhanced recognition of novel symbols (Longcamp et al, 2008;James and Atwood, 2009;Li and James, 2016). Previous fMRI studies that have investigated the neural mechanisms underlying such learning have found enhanced activation in visual cortex when viewing previously practiced letters (James and Gauthier, 2006;James, 2017), and increased connectivity between visual and parietal regions following handwriting experience (Vinci-Booher et al, 2016). However, these studies have focused on univariate measures of BOLD signal amplitude within regions or when analyzing connectivity, raising the question of whether these changes reflect the recruitment of similar representations across tasks or of colocated but functionally distinct representations for each task.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Such enhanced transmission may reflect participants’ increasing ability to emphasize the diagnostic features of each object across repeated attempts to transform their perceptual representation of the object into an effective motor plan to draw it. More broadly, these findings sugges that the manner in which information is communicated between sensory and downstream regions may be a potential neural substrate for learning complex visually guided actions, including visual production (Vinci-Booher et al, 2016).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…These findings resonate with other evidence that visual production can support learning, including maintenance of recently learned information (Peynircioğlu, 1989; Wammes, Meade, & Fernandes, 2016) and enhanced recognition of novel symbols (Longcamp et al, 2008; James & Atwood, 2009; Li & James, 2016). Previous fMRI studies that have investigated the neural mechanisms underlying such learning have found enhanced activation in visual cortex when viewing previously practiced letters (James & Gauthier, 2006; James, 2017), and increased connectivity between visual and parietal regions following handwriting experience (Vinci-Booher, James, & James, 2016). However, these studies have focused on univariate measures of BOLD signal amplitude within regions or when analyzing connectivity, raising the question of whether these changes reflect the recruitment of similar representations across tasks or of co-located but functionally distinct representations for each task.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this respect, handwriting and typing have fundamentally different properties (Mangen and Velay, 2010;Mangen and Balsvik, 2016): Handwriting requires carefully reproducing the shape of each letter, whereas in typewriting the motor program is not related to the letter shape and, as a result, no such grapho-motor component is present. Hence, motor programs associated with handwriting provide an additional informative memory trace and may contribute to the representation of the shape of a letter (James and Engelhardt, 2012;Vinci-Booher et al, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When novel letters were trained by handwriting, an activation in motor areas was observed, which was absent when these novel letters were trained by typing (James and Atwood, 2008;Longcamp et al, 2008). Furthermore, handwriting experience also seems to be necessary in children to develop the adult-like neuronal circuit of letter processing (James and Engelhardt, 2012) by increasing functionally connectivity between visual and motor brain regions (Vinci-Booher et al, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%