2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.10.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual perception of planar-rotated 2D objects in goldfish (Carassius auratus)

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
3

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

2
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
1
17
3
Order By: Relevance
“…However, our dolphin showed the lowest performance at 180°, which contrasts with the sea lion in Mauck and Dehnhardt’s (1997) study showing lowest performance at 90° (but highest RTs at 180°). Our results contrast with studies that have not shown systematic decrements in performance with planar-rotated 2-D stimuli as a function of aspect angle (fish: DeLong et al, 2018; lion-tailed macaques: Burmann et al, 2005). Our results showing that performance with planar-rotated objects was significantly better at 0° than 180° agree with other studies.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, our dolphin showed the lowest performance at 180°, which contrasts with the sea lion in Mauck and Dehnhardt’s (1997) study showing lowest performance at 90° (but highest RTs at 180°). Our results contrast with studies that have not shown systematic decrements in performance with planar-rotated 2-D stimuli as a function of aspect angle (fish: DeLong et al, 2018; lion-tailed macaques: Burmann et al, 2005). Our results showing that performance with planar-rotated objects was significantly better at 0° than 180° agree with other studies.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…The ability to perceive visually objects rotated in the picture or depth planes has been investigated in a wide variety of nonhuman animals that live in terrestrial habitats (rats: Minini & Jeffery, 2006; Sutherland, 1969; ferrets: Pollard, Beale, Lysons, & Preston, 1967; sheep: Kendrick, Atkins, Hinton, Heavens, & Keverne, 1996; newborn chicks: Wood, 2013; dogs: Racca et al, 2010; and baboons: Hopkins, Fagot, & Vauclair, 1993), arboreal habitats (monkeys: Freedman, Riesenhuber, Poggio, & Miller, 2006; Köhler, Hoffmann, Dehnhardt, & Mauck, 2005; Logothetis, Pauls, Bülthoff, & Poggio, 1994; Nielsen et al, 2008; Parr, 2011; Parr & Heintz, 2008; lion-tailed macaques: Burmann, Dehnhardt, & Mauck, 2005; and chimpanzees: Parr, 2011), aerial habitats (pigeons: Cook & Katz, 1999; Delius & Hollard, 1995; Hamm, Matheson, & Honig, 1997; Hollard & Delius, 1982; Jitsumori & Ohkubo, 1996; Spetch, Friedman, & Reid, 2001; Wasserman et al, 1996; honeybees: Dyer & Vuong, 2008; Plowright et al, 2001), and aquatic habitats (sea lions: Mauck & Dehnhardt, 1997; Schusterman & Thomas, 1966; octopus: Sutherland, 1969; fish: Bowman & Sutherland, 1969; DeLong, Fobe, O’Leary, & Wilcox, 2018; Schluessel, Kraniotakes, & Bleckmann, 2014; Wang & Takeuchi, 2017). The results of such studies can vary based on stimulus type (simple or complex), task (match-to-sample, forced choice paradigm, go/no-go, same-different task), type of training (one view vs. multiple views), and rotation plane.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An additional important study of object recognition in teleosts was performed in the goldfish, where fish were able to identify a handful of single objects irrespective of rotation ( DeLong et al, 2018 ). Our study extends this finding by showing that teleostei are able to categorize and overcome transformation of translation, rotation, size (or taken together, what is known as similarity transformations) and contrast.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of studies have indicated that fish also have the capacity to differentiate between different shapes ( Lucon-Xiccato et al, 2019 ; Mackintosh and Sutherland, 1963 ; May et al, 2016 ; Oliveira et al, 2015 ; Siebeck et al, 2009 ) and intensities ( Agrillo et al, 2016 ), use hierarchy of visual processing in discriminating shapes ( Truppa et al, 2010 ) and fish faces ( Parker et al, 2020 ), and the archerfish can even be trained to discriminate between human faces ( Newport et al, 2016 , 2018 ). For simple abstract stimuli, fish are capable of overcoming changes in orientation ( DeLong et al, 2018 ), size consistency ( Douglas et al, 1988 ) and the occlusion of objects ( Sovrano and Bisazza, 2008 ). In addition to these findings in teleosts, there is an interesting study in sharks, showing they were able to discriminate teleost fish from snails ( Schluessel and Düngen, 2015 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[23], [30] Movement may have altered the orientation of the initial record, so effective identification of the shape requires rotation invariance. [31], [32] Further, the entire complement of boundary markers for a given shape may not all be present due to occlusion. If the object is hidden behind a dense thicket of sea-plants or coral, only fragmented portions of the boundary may be visible at a given moment (see Figure 6).…”
Section: Elementary Shape Filtersmentioning
confidence: 99%