1985
DOI: 10.1080/00221325.1985.9914466
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual Perspective Taking in Children: Further Ramifications of an Information-Processing Model

Abstract: Forty children between ages 6 and 8 were administered a set of spatial perspective tasks. On half of the items, children responded by rotating a duplicate of the target display; on the remainder, children reconstructed the displays to correspond to a perspective view. The displays differed as to whether they contained marked or unmarked objects. On the basis of an information-processing analysis of these tasks, we predicted that the response-type variables and stimulus variables would interact in known ways. A… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the famous three mountains task, children of various ages were asked to judge what another agent could see from their viewpoint. This initiated an abundance of work assessing, for example, how perspective-taking relates to broader developmental stages [ 2 ] and how familiarity with the display items modulates responses [ 3 ]. The visual perspective-taking notion has also been applied to many non-developmental areas, such as ‘joint action’ in which two participants sit adjacently and share responses (i.e., take turns) on a target discrimination task (for a review, see [ 4 ]).…”
Section: Perspective-takingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the famous three mountains task, children of various ages were asked to judge what another agent could see from their viewpoint. This initiated an abundance of work assessing, for example, how perspective-taking relates to broader developmental stages [ 2 ] and how familiarity with the display items modulates responses [ 3 ]. The visual perspective-taking notion has also been applied to many non-developmental areas, such as ‘joint action’ in which two participants sit adjacently and share responses (i.e., take turns) on a target discrimination task (for a review, see [ 4 ]).…”
Section: Perspective-takingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is because the information processing abilities involved in these spatial perspective taking tasks vary and considerably influence the accuracy of task performance (Newcombe, 1989). Information processing can refer to the perception of object features (Rosser, Ensing, Mazzeo, & Horan, 1985), object-manipulation ability (Kozhevnikov & Hegarty, 2001), inhibitory functions (Hasher & Zacks, 1988), or working memory (Yamadori, Ashina, Fujii, Tsukiura, Okuda, & Osaka, 1999). Hegarty and Waller (2004) proposed a mental model of spatial perspective taking in which imagining oneself reoriented at another point in space should be separated from the other information processing abilities that eventually generate the view from this new vantage point.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In recent years, Hegarty and Wallen (2004) proposed a mental model of spatial perspective-taking in which they separated imaginary body movement from other cognitive information processes. Cognitive information processing-other than that relating to imaginary body movement-could be considered to refer to the recognition of object features (Rosser, Ensing, Mazzeo, & Horan, 1985), object-manipulation spatial ability (Kozhevnikov & Hegarty, 2001), inhibitory functions (Hasher & Zacks, 1988), or simply working memory (Yamadori, Ashina, Fujii, Tsukiura, Okuda, & Osaka, 1999). Even if a person carries out imaginary body movement correctly, his/her responses to a task will contain errors if the subsequent cognitive information processing is inadequate.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%