2007
DOI: 10.1080/13506280600871917
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual short-term memory: Central capacity limitations in short-term consolidation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

10
92
1
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 67 publications
(105 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
10
92
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…While the aforementioned considerations suggest that it is plausible that the consolidation of a visual stimulus can be disrupted by means of a trailing 2-AFC task, it is important to note that this type of retroactive interference effect has thus far not been observed in studies that combined a memory encoding task with a trailing 2-AFC task (Arnell & Duncan, 2002;Arnell et al, 2004;Jolicoeur & Dell'Acqua, 1998;Koch & Rumiati, 2006;Ruthruff & Pashler, 2001;Stevanovski & Jolicoeur, 2007Tombu et al, 2011). To be precise, these studies all found that performance on the 2-AFC task suffers a pronounced proactive interference effect in the form of a psychological refractory period effect (i.e., a decrease in response times with increasing stimulus onset asynchrony [SOA]; Welford, 1952;see also Pashler, 1994), whereas performance on the memory task showed little to no evidence for a concomitant retroactive interference effect.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…While the aforementioned considerations suggest that it is plausible that the consolidation of a visual stimulus can be disrupted by means of a trailing 2-AFC task, it is important to note that this type of retroactive interference effect has thus far not been observed in studies that combined a memory encoding task with a trailing 2-AFC task (Arnell & Duncan, 2002;Arnell et al, 2004;Jolicoeur & Dell'Acqua, 1998;Koch & Rumiati, 2006;Ruthruff & Pashler, 2001;Stevanovski & Jolicoeur, 2007Tombu et al, 2011). To be precise, these studies all found that performance on the 2-AFC task suffers a pronounced proactive interference effect in the form of a psychological refractory period effect (i.e., a decrease in response times with increasing stimulus onset asynchrony [SOA]; Welford, 1952;see also Pashler, 1994), whereas performance on the memory task showed little to no evidence for a concomitant retroactive interference effect.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To be precise, there are several neuroimaging studies that show that WMC relies on a network of frontal and parietal brain areas that are also activated in tasks that require participants to decide upon and select an appropriate response for a stimulus (Marti, Sigman, & Dehaene, 2012;Tombu et al, 2011; see also Marois & Ivanoff, 2005;Zylberberg, Slezak, Roelfsema, Dehaene, & Sigman, 2010). Furthermore, there are several behavioral studies that suggest that these two types of tasks produce interference when they need to be performed for two targets shown in close temporal succession (e.g., Arnell & Duncan, 2002;Arnell, Helion, Hurdelbrink, & Pasieka, 2004;De Jong & Sweet, 1994;Jolicoeur & Dell'Acqua, 1998Koch & Rumiati, 2006;Ruthruff & Pashler, 2001;Stevanovski & Jolicoeur, 2007see also Wong, 2002). Accordingly, we reasoned that if the consolidation of a visual stimulus continues after a mask, then it might still suffer interference from a trailing 2-AFC task because the execution of this task would call upon the same central processing resources that mediate the ongoing consolidation of the earlier shown stimuli.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations